Abstract
Recent research in American political behavior has examined at length the link between evangelical Protestants and the Republican Party. These works however do not consider the idiosyncratic nature of religiosity in the US, and insist on treating religion as an ‘unmoved mover’ with respect to political contexts. The question posed herein is: during the participation of religious communities in partisan politics, should we expect politics to eventually constrain religious behavior? Motivated by a political social identity approach, I use American National Election Study panel data and structural equation modeling techniques to explore the untested possibility that religious and political factors are linked through reciprocal causation. Conditional upon religious and temporal context, findings highlight the causal impact of ideology and partisanship in shaping religious behavior.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
This stereotyping might seem a simplification of reality—for example, not all Republicans or conservatives are evangelicals. SIT stresses the point that members perceive in- and out-groups as homogenous, and not that they actually embody these stereotypes (Huddy 2003).
Data used in the present study were made available by the Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research. The author holds sole responsibility for their analysis and interpretation. Results from supplementary analyses mentioned in the text are available from the author. Throughout the study, I avoid using the 1956–1960 ANES panel, since the detailed measure that differentiates among Protestant denominations and is required for sub-group analysis had not yet been introduced in 1956. Also, in the examination of the reciprocal link between ideology and attendance, the 2000–2004 panel is dropped, since there is no repeated measure of ideological self-placement in 2004.
Note that the item may be subject to misreporting due to social desirability effects, a problem however that cannot be corrected with ANES data (e.g. Hadaway et al. 1993).
Establishment of causal precedence is possible with cross-sectional data, when simultaneous effects are assumed between two variables (Finkel 1995). This however requires the use of unrealistic modeling options in the form of instrumental variables. According to theory, instruments should be very strong predictors of one endogenous variable, but not related to the second endogenous variable. Instrumental variables are difficult to locate in social surveys, unless designers had planned ahead and included such indicators in the questionnaire.
FIML does not exclude missing cases from analysis for respondents included in the models. Under the assumption of data missing at random, the procedure produces superior estimates to either listwise or pairwise deletion, or mean imputation (Arbuckle 2005). Regarding panel effects, Bartels’ study (1999) shows that the ANES design does not suffer from serious panel attrition and conditioning, with the exception of campaign interest and turnout variables.
Steensland et al. classify nondenominational Protestants as evangelicals according to their church attendance (frequent attendance suggests evangelicalism) (2000, p. 316). Since the present analysis is built on the church attendance variable, adopting the above practice would have introduced a biased logic: religiosity would feature both as an independent/dependent variable within religious groups and as a stratification criterion across groups. Facing the risk of introducing an amount of unwanted variability in the groupings, nondenominational Protestants were assigned to the evangelical group in recent decades, irrespective of their observance.
The effects reported in this paper hold even with more homogenous stratifications. For instance, if models are estimated within born-again evangelicals (measure not available in 1972–1976), the ‘partisan religion’ effect is intensified. I do not emphasize this result however, for two main reasons. First, subsample size further decreases. Second, the ‘born-again’ item is a subjective measure, less reliable than denominational membership. Estimates are insensitive to an additional test: the successive dropping of each control variable from the analysis.
In an earlier stage of the analysis, I used three-wave models with the 1972–1976 and 2000–2004 datasets, which employed a Wiley-Wiley specification with single-indicator latent variables. These produced a similar picture of causal relationships as the one presented in this study. However, in some cases AMOS returned inadmissible figures, such as negative variances. I attribute this to small and homogenous subsamples, which possibly hindered the efficient estimation of complex models with latent variables.
Goodness-of-fit is assessed with the following criteria (Arbuckle 2005): the χ2 test/degrees of freedom ratio, in which values less than 5 are desirable (or 3 for stricter evaluations); Bollen’s incremental fit index (IFI), which makes adjustments for sample size and the complexity of the model, taking into account degrees of freedom, should score close to 90 and above (or .95 and above for more conservative evaluations); Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI), which again accounts for small sample sizes and should be greater than .90 (or .95 for stricter evaluations); finally, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), where values should be lower than .08 (or .05 for more conservative evaluations). All models tested—feedback and unidirectional, for all groups, during all periods—had an acceptable fit to the variances/covariances encountered in the data.
According to the test in Table 4, a partisan religion also emerges among mainline Protestants in the 1970s, whereby partisanship appears to constrain church attendance. One explanation of this could lie with the political mobilization of mainline churches during the turbulent 1960s and 1970s, as defined by the Civil Rights movement and the Vietnam War (e.g. Wald and Calhoun-Brown 2007). Yet, due to methodological problems in model estimation for this subsample in the 1970s, I choose instead to emphasize the absence of the political religion effect from the evangelical subsample, which is the focus of the present study. Specifically, the cross-effects have different signs for mainline Protestants in the 1972–1976 model: lagged partisanship has a positive effect on changing attendance, while lagged attendance has a counter-intuitive, negative effect on changing partisanship (see Supplementary electronic material, Table C). This is a suppressor effect, whereby two variables are positively correlated, but direct effects are negative or vice versa. Suppression occurs for two main reasons (Smith et al. 1992). First, suppression happens when an additional predictor is entered in the model, i.e. the true relationship between the variables is in the opposite direction than the one indicated by their correlation. If the difference in sign can be explained theoretically, the effect can be retained. Second, suppression can be the result of multicollinearity, an inherent problem in cross-lagged models, which use repeated measures of often very stables variables. The problem is aggravated here by the stratification of the sample into homogenous subsamples. I find the second explanation more plausible. This justifies my concern in reading too much into this result.
Note that the detection of significant effects especially post-1990s obtains greater leverage due to small subsample sizes (higher probability for a Type II Error).
To test whether interactions are significant, i.e. whether effects differ across religious communities, I fit two models for each panel: first I allow all parameters to differ across groups. In the second case, I constrain cross-lagged effects to be equal across groups. The relative validity of the two assumptions can be evaluated with a χ2 difference test, i.e. the difference in χ2 values between the two hypotheses. This will indicate which model fits the data better (Bollen 1989, p. 292). Results show that the intergroup differences observed in Tables 2 and 4 are significant.
References
Abramowitz, A., & Saunders, K. (1998). Ideological realignment in the U.S. electorate. Journal of Politics, 60, 634–652.
Aldrich, J. H. (1995). Why parties? The origin and transformation of party politics in America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Arbuckle, J. (2005). Amos 6.0 user’s guide. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Corporation.
Asher, H. (1983). Causal modeling. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Bartels, L. M. (1999). Panel effects in the American national election studies. Political Analysis, 8, 1–20.
Bellah, R. (1967). Civil religion in America. Daedalus, 96, 1–21.
Bolzendahl, C., & Brooks, C. (2005). Polarization, secularization or differences as usual? The denominational cleavage in U.S. social attitudes since the 1970s. Sociological Quarterly, 46, 47–78.
Bollen, K. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.
Campbell, A. (1960). Surge and decline: A study of electoral change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 397–418.
Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American voter. New York: Wiley.
Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1966). Elections & the political order. NY: Wiley.
Carmines, E., & Stimson, J. (1989). Issue evolution. Princeton: Princeton UP.
Carsey, T., & Layman, G. (2006). Changing sides or changing minds? Party identification and policy preferences in the American electorate. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 464–477.
Clarke, H., & Stewart, M. (1998). The decline of parties in the minds of citizens. Annual Review of Political Science, 1, 357–378.
Davidson, R., & Oleszek, W. (2004). Congress and its members (9th ed.). Washington: CQ Press.
Deaux, K, Reid, A., Mizrahi, K., & Ethier K. (1995). Parameters of social identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 280–291.
Edelman, M. (1964). The symbolic uses of politics. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Finke, R., & Stark, R. (1992). The churching of America, 1776–1990: Winners and losers in our religious economy. New Brunswick: Rutgers UP.
Finkel, S. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Fiorina, M. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. Yale UP.
Goren, P. (2005). Party identification and core political values. American Journal of Political Science, 49, 881–896.
Green, D., & Palmquist, B. (1990). Of artifacts and partisan instability. American Journal of Political Science, 34, 872–902.
Green, D., Palmquist B., & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan hearts and minds: Political parties and the social identities of voters. New Haven: Yale UP.
Green, J. C., Rozell, M., & Wilcox, C. (Eds.). (2003). The Christian right in American politics: marching to the millennium. Washington: Georgetown UP.
Greene, S. (1999). Understanding party identification: A social identity approach. Political Psychology, 20, 393–403.
Greene, S. (2002). The social-psychological measurement of partisanship. Political Behavior, 24, 171–197.
Greene, S. (2004). Social identity theory and party identification. Social Science Quarterly, 85, 136–153.
Guth, J. L., Green, J. C., Smidt, C., Kellstedt, L., & Poloma, M. (1997). The bully pulpit: The politics of Protestant clergy. Lawrence: University of Kansas Press.
Hadaway, K., Marler, P. L., & Chaves, M. (1993). What the polls don’t show: A closer look at church attendance. American Sociological Review, 58, 741–752.
Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Social identity and self-categorization processes in organizational contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25, 121–140.
Hout, M., & Fischer, C. (2002). Why more Americans have no religious preference. American Sociological Review, 67, 165–190.
Huddy, L. (2003). Group identity and political cohesion. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford UP.
Jackson, J. E. (1975). Issues, party choices and presidential votes. American Journal of Political Science, 19, 161–186.
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NY: Guilford.
Kriesi, H. (1998). The transformation of cleavage politics. The 1997 Stein Rokkan Lecture. European Journal of Political Research, 33, 165–185.
Lau, R. (1989). Individual and contextual influences on group identification. Social Psychology Quarterly, 52, 220–231.
Layman, G. (1997). Religion and political behavior in the United States: The impact of beliefs, affiliations, and commitment from 1980 to 1994. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 61, 288–316.
Layman, G. (2001). The great divide: Religious and cultural conflict in American party politics. New York: Columbia UP.
Layman, G., & Carsey, T. (2002). Party polarization and party structuring of policy attitudes: A comparison of three NES panel studies. Political Behavior, 24, 199–236.
Layman, G., & Green, J. C. (2005). Wars and rumours of wars: The contexts of cultural conflict in American political behaviour. British Journal of Political Science, 36, 61–89.
Layman, G., & Hussey, L. (2005). George W. Bush and the Evangelicals. Presented at the Conference on ‘A Matter of Faith? Religion and the 2004 Presidential Election,’ University of Notre Dame.
Leege, D. C., & Kellstedt, L. A. (Eds.). (1993). Rediscovering the religious factor in American politics. Armonk, N.Y.: M.E. Sharpe.
Lenski, G. (1963). The Religious Factor. NY: Doubleday.
Levendusky, M. (2005). Sorting in the U.S. mass electorate. Presented at the MPSA Annual Meeting.
Lipset, S. M., & Rokkan, S. (1967). Cleavage structures, party systems, and voter alignments: An introduction. In S. M. Lipset, & S. Rokkan (Eds.), Party systems and voter alignments. NY: Free Press.
Long, K. M., & Spears, R. (1997). The self-esteem hypothesis revisited: Differentiation and the disaffected. In R. Spears, P. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyping and group life. Oxford: Blackwell.
Loveland, M. T. (2003). Religious switching: Preference development, maintenance, and change. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42, 147–157.
Manza, J., & Brooks, C. (1999). Social cleavages and political change. Oxford: Oxford UP.
Markus, G, & Converse, P. (1979). A dynamic simultaneous equation model of electoral choice. American Political Science Review, 73, 1055–1070.
Miller, W. E., & Shanks, J. M. (1996). The new American voter. Cambridge: Harvard UP.
Moen, M. C. (1994). From revolution to evolution: The changing nature of the Christian right. Sociology of Religion, 55, 345–357.
Newport, F. (1979). The religious switcher in the US. American Sociological Review, 44, 528–552.
Nie, N., Verba, S., & Petrocik, J. (1979). The changing American voter. Cambridge: Harvard UP.
Page, B., & Jones, C. (1979). Reciprocal effects of policy preferences, party loyalties, and the vote. American Political Science Review, 73, 1071–1090.
Poole, K. T., & Rosenthal, H. (1984). The polarization of American politics. Journal of Politics, 46, 1061–1079.
Roof, W. C., & McKinney, W. (1987). American mainline religion: Its changing shape and future. New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers UP.
Sartori, G. (1969). From the sociology of politics to political sociology. In S. M. Lipset (Ed.), Politics and the social sciences. NY: Oxford UP.
Smith, R., Ager, J., & Williams, D. (1992). Suppressor variables in multiple regression/correlation. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 17–29.
Steensland, B., Park, J., Regnerus, M., Robinson, L., Wilcox, W. B., & Woodberry, R. (2000). The measure of American religion: Toward improving the state of the art. Social Forces, 79, 291–318.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.
Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior. In E. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes: Theory and research Vol. 2. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Wald, K., & Calhoun-Brown, A. (2007). Religion and politics in the United States (5th ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Warner, R. S. (1993). Work in progress toward a new paradigm for the sociological study of religion in the United States. American Journal of Sociology, 98, 1044–1093.
Wilcox, C. (1996). Onward Christian soldiers: The religious right in American politics. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge UP.
Acknowledgments
Earlier versions of the paper were presented at the Joint Sessions of the European Consortium for Political Research (Workshop: Politicizing Socio-Cultural Structures), Helsinki, May 2007, and the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Religion, Tampa, November 2007. I am indebted to Mark Shephard, John Curtice, Robert Johns, the editors and two anonymous reviewers for advice and comments. I also want to thank Christopher J. Carman and Wouter van der Brug for helpful suggestions.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
With apologies to Alexis de Tocqueville.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Patrikios, S. American Republican Religion? Disentangling the Causal Link Between Religion and Politics in the US. Polit Behav 30, 367–389 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9053-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9053-1