Adams, F., & Mele, A. (1989). The role of intention in intentional action. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 19, 511–531
Google Scholar
Alvarez, M., & Hyman, J. (1998). Agents and their actions. Philosophy, 73, 219–245
Google Scholar
Anderson, B. (2013). A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. Journal of Vision, 13(3), 1–16
Google Scholar
Ansorge, U., Kunde, W., & Kiefer, M. (2014). Unconscious vision and executive control: how unconscious processing and conscious action control interact. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 268–287
Google Scholar
Awh, E., Belopolsky, A., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-Down vs. Bottom-Up Attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437–443
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Bays, P., Gorgoraptis, N., Wee, N., Marshall, L., & Husain, M. (2011). Temporal dynamics of encoding, storage, and reallocation of visual working memory. Journal of Vision, 11, 1–15
Google Scholar
Bechtel, W. (2008). Mental Mechanisms: Philosophical Perspectives on Cognitive Neuroscience. Routledge.
Google Scholar
Beck, V., Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. (2012). Simultaneous control of attention by multiple working memory representations. Psychological science, 23(8), 887–898
Google Scholar
Bishop, J. (1989). Natural Agency. An Essay on the Causal Theory of Action. Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (2014). The computational and neural basis of cognitive control: charted territory and new frontiers. Cognitive Science, 38, 1249–1285
Google Scholar
Brand, M. (1984). Intending and Acting. MIT.
Google Scholar
Burge, T. (2009). Primitive agency and natural norms. Philosophy and phenomenological research, 79, 251–278
Google Scholar
Burge, T. (2010). Origins of objectivity. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (2008). Interventionism, control variables, and causation in the qualitative world. Philosophical issues, 18, 426–445
Google Scholar
Carlisle, N., Arita, J., Pardo, D., & Woodman, G. (2011). Attentional templates in visual working memory. Journal of neuroscience, 31, 9315–9322
Google Scholar
Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51, 1484–1525
Google Scholar
Chisholm, R. (1964). The descriptive element in the concept of action. Journal of Philosophy, 61, 613–625
Google Scholar
Chun, M. (2003). Scene Perception and Memory. In D. Irwin & B. Ross (Eds.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory: Cognitive Vision. (Vol. 42, pp. 79–108). Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Craver, C. (2007). Explaining the Brain. Mechanisms and the Mosaic Unity of Neuroscience. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Cummins, R. (1983). The nature of psychological explanation. MIT.
Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1971). Agency. In D. Davidson (Ed.), 1980, Essays on actions and events.Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1973). Freedom to Act. In D. Davidson (Ed.), 1980, Essays on actions and events.Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1969). Content and consciousness. Routledge & Kegan.
Google Scholar
D’Esposito, M. (2007). From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 362(1481), 761–772
Google Scholar
D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142
Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168
Google Scholar
Drayson, Z. (2012). The uses and abuses of the personal/sub-personal distinction. Philosophical Perspectives, 26(1), 1–18
Google Scholar
Drayson, Z. (2014). The personal/sub-personal distinction. Philosophy compass, 9(5), 338–346
Google Scholar
Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96(3), 433–458
Google Scholar
Enc, B. (2003). How We Act: Causes, Reasons, and Intentions. OUP.
Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Frankfurt, H. 1978. “The problem of action” in H. Frankfurt (1988), The importance of what we care about, CUP
Fuster, J. (2015). The prefrontal cortex. Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M., Ivry, R., & Mangun, G. (2014). Cognitive Neuroscience. The Biology of the Mind. Norton.
Google Scholar
Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J., Princiotta, D., & Otero, T. (2014). A history of executive functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct. In S. Goldstein & J. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning.Springer.
Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1970). A Theory of Human Action. Prentice Hall.
Google Scholar
Han, S. (2015). Working memory contents revive the neglected, but suppress the inhibited. Cognition, 145, 116–121
Google Scholar
Han, S., & Kim, M. (2004). Visual search does not remain efficient when executive working memory is working. Psychological Science, 15(9), 623–628
Google Scholar
Han, S., & Kim, M. (2009). Do the contents of working memory capture attention? Yes, but cognitive control matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 1292–1302
Google Scholar
Hollingworth, A. 2014. “Guidance of Visual Search by Memory and Knowledge.” In M. Dodd & J. Flowers (eds.), The Influence of Attention, Learning, and Motivation on Visual Search, Nebraska Symposion on Motivation
Hornsby, J. (1980). Actions. Routledge.
Google Scholar
Hornsby, J. (1996). Simple Mindedness. In Defense of Naïve Naturalism in the Philosophy of Mind. Harvard.
Google Scholar
Hornsby, J. (2000). Personal and sub-personal: A defense of Dennett’s early distinction. Philosophical Explorations, 3(1), 6–24
Google Scholar
Hornsby, J. (2004). Agency and Alienation. In M. Caro & D. MacArthur (Eds.), Naturalism in Question.Cambridge: Harvard.
Google Scholar
Hyman, J. (2015). Action, Knowledge, and Will. OUP.
Google Scholar
Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Phillip, A., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching –a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874
Google Scholar
Kim, J. (1998). Mind in a physical world. An essay on the mind-body problem and mental causation. MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Kristjansson, A., & Campana, G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: a review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(1), 5–18
Google Scholar
Lavie, N., & Dalton, P. (2014). The load theory of attention and cognitive control. In A. Nobre & S. Kastner (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Attention.Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Lu, J., Tian, L., Zhang, J., Wang, J., Ye, C., & Liu, Q. (2017). Strategic inhibition of distractors with visual working memory contents after involuntary attention capture. Nature scientific reports, 7, 1–9
Google Scholar
Mele, A. (1992). The springs of action. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Mele, A. (2000). Goal-directed action: Teleological Explanations, Causal Theories, and Deviance. Philosophical Perspectives, 14, 279–300
Google Scholar
Miller, E., & Cohen, J. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 4, 167–202
Google Scholar
Miller, E., T. J., & Buschman. (2013). Cortical circuits for the control of attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 216–222
Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100
Google Scholar
Mole, C. (2011). Attention is Cognitive Unison. OUP.
Google Scholar
Munakata, Y., Hard, S., Chatham, C., Depue, B., Banich, M., & O’Reilly, R. (2011). A unified frameworke for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 453–459
Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1986). The View from Nowhere. OUP.
Google Scholar
Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and Self-regulation: Advances in Research. (Vol. IV, pp. 1–18). Plenum Press.
Google Scholar
O’Connor, T. (2000). Persons and Causes: The Metaphysics of Free Will. OUP.
Google Scholar
O’Shaughnessy, B. (1980). The Will. CUP.
Google Scholar
Olivers, C., & Eimer, M. (2011). On the difference between working memory and attentional set. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1553–1558
Google Scholar
Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107(1), 179–217
Google Scholar
Peacocke, C. (1979). Deviant causal chains. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 4, 123–155
Google Scholar
Peacocke, C. 2007. “Mental Action and Self-Awareness (I).” in J. Cohen & B. McLaughlin (2007), Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Mind, Blackwell
Pereboom, D. (2004). Is our conception of agent-causation coherent? Philosophical Topics, 32, 275–286
Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2012). The Conscious Brain. OUP.
Google Scholar
Posner, M. & Snyder, C. 1975. “Attention and cognitive control.” In Solso, Information processing and cognition, Erlbaum
Posner, M. (1980). Orienting of Attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25
Google Scholar
Rougier, N., Noelle, D., Bravier, T., Cohen, J., & O’Reilly, R. (2005). Prefrontal cortex and flexible cognitive control: rules without symbols. PNAS, 102(20), 7338–7343
Google Scholar
Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. (2011). Active suppression of distractors that match the content of visual working memory. Visual cognition, 19, 956–972
Google Scholar
Schlosser, M. (2007). Basic deviance reconsidered. Analysis, 67(3), 186–194
Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. (2014). The contours of control. Philosophical Studies, 170(3), 395–411
Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic information processing: II. Perception, Learning, Automatic Attending, and a General Theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190
Google Scholar
Smithies, D. 2011a “Attention is Rational-Access Consciousness,” in C. Mole, D. Smithies & W. Wu (eds.), Attention. Philosophical and Psychological Essays, OUP: New York
Soto, D., Humphreys, G., Heinke, D., & Blanco, M. (2005). Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31(2), 248–261
Google Scholar
Steward, H. (2012). A Metaphysics of Freedom. OUP.
Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1978). Beliefs and subdoxastic states. Philosophy of science, 45, 499–518
Google Scholar
Thalberg, I. (1984). Do our intentions cause our intentional actions? American Philosophical Quarterly, 21, 249–260
Google Scholar
Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-Down and Bottom-Up Control of Visual Selection. Acta Psychologica, 135, 77–99
Google Scholar
Torralba, A., Oliva, A., Castelhano, M., & Henderson, J. (2006). Contextual guidance of attention in natural scenes: The role of global features on object search. Psychological Review, 113(4), 766–786
Google Scholar
Tsotsos, J., & Kruijne, W. (2014). Cognitive programs: software for attention’s executive. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 1–16
Google Scholar
Velleman, D. 1992. “What happens when someone acts?.” In D. Velleman (2000), The possibility of practical reason. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Library
Vickery, T., King, L., & Jiang, Y. (2005). Setting up the target template in visual search. Journal of vision, 5, 81–92
Google Scholar
Vogel, E., Woodman, G., & Luck, S. (2001). Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual working memory. Journal of experimental psychology, 27, 92–114
Google Scholar
Walker, R., & McSorley, E. (2008). The Influence of distractors on saccade-target selection: Saccade trajectory effects. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(3), 1–13
Google Scholar
Watzl, S. (2017). Structuring mind: the nature of attention and how it shapes consciousness. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Weiskopf, D. (2011). Models and mechanisms in psychological explanation. Synthese, 183(3), 313–338
Google Scholar
Weiskopf, D. (2018). The Explanatory Autonomy of Cognitive Models. In M. Kaplan (Ed.), Integrating Psychology and Neuroscience: Prospects and Problems.Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. (1997). Computational approaches to motor control. Trends in Cognitive Motor Sciences, 1(6), 209–216
Google Scholar
Wright, R., & Ward, L. (2008). Orienting of Attention. Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2014). Attention. Routledge.
Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2016). Experts and deviants: The story of agentive control. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 93(1), 101–126
Google Scholar
Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 121–134
Google Scholar
Zelinsky, G. (2008). A Theory of eye movements during target acquisition. Psychological Review, 115(4), 787–835
Google Scholar
Zelinsky, G. (2015). The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory. Annals of the new York Academy of Sciences, 1339(1), 154–164
Google Scholar