Advertisement

Philosophical Studies

, Volume 168, Issue 1, pp 151–165 | Cite as

Content and context in incremental processing: “the ham sandwich” revisited

  • Petra B. SchumacherEmail author
Article

Abstract

The interplay of content and context is observable in a moment to moment manner as propositional content unfolds. The current contribution illustrates this through data from real-time language comprehension indicating that propositional content is not computed in isolation but relies in important ways on context during every step of the computation of meaning. The relevant notion of context that we have to adopt includes all aspects of possible worlds and draws on a variety of knowledge representations, which in a first processing phase serve to generate expectations for upcoming words. In a second phase, the discourse representation is assessed and if necessary updated by means of inferential reasoning and enrichment to reflect the speaker’s intended meaning.

Keywords

Pragmatics Meaning shift Context Speaker meaning Inference 

Notes

Acknowledgments

A version of this work has been presented at the Content, Context and Conversation Workshop at the Lichtenberg-Kolleg of the Georg-August University Göttingen in 2011. I would like to thank the organizers, Magdalena Kaufmann, Christian Beyer and Markus Steinbach, and the workshop audience for inspiring discussion. The research on meaning shift was carried out as part of a project funded by the German Research Foundation (BU 1853/2-1). I am grateful to Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky (at that time at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig) for generously providing lab space to conduct this study in collaboration with the Clinic for Audiology and Phoniatry (Prof. Manfred Gross) of the Charité Berlin, and Katja Bruening, Elisabeth Dietz, Jona Sassenhagen, and Jan Patrick Zeller for their assistance at various stages of data preparation, collection and analysis.

References

  1. Altmann, G., & Steedman, M. (1988). Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition, 30(3), 191–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Asher, N. (2011). Lexical meaning in context: A web of words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Asher, N., & Lascarides, A. (1983). Bridging. Journal of Semantics, 15, 83–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bach, K. (2005). Context ex machina. In Z. Szabó (Ed.), Semantics versus pragmatics (pp. 15–44). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bosco, F. M., Bucciarelli, M., & Bara, B. G. (2004). The fundamental context categories in understanding communicative intention. Journal of Pragmatics, 36(3), 467–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding—Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 717–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Burkhardt, P. (2006). Inferential bridging relations reveal distinct neural mechanisms: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain and Language, 98(2), 159–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Burkhardt, P. (2007). The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. NeuroReport, 18(17), 1851–1854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chafe, W. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 25–55). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  10. Copestake, A., & Briscoe, T. (1995). Semi-productive polysemy and sense extension. Journal of Semantics, 12, 15–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Coulson, S., & van Petten, C. (2002). Conceptual integration and metaphor: An event-related potential study. Memory and Cognition, 30(6), 958–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Egg, M. (2004). Metonymie als Phänomen der Semantik-Pragmatik-Schnittstelle. Metaphorik, 6, 36–53.Google Scholar
  13. Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferraro, V. (2002). Good-enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 11–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (1990). Taking on semantic commitments—Processing multiple meanings vs. multiple senses. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(2), 181–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Frisson, S. (2009). Semantic underspecification in language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 111–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Frisson, S., & Pickering, M. J. (1999). The processing of metonymy: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6), 1366–1383.Google Scholar
  17. Frisson, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). The processing of familiar and novel senses of a word: Why reading Dickens is easy but reading Needham can be hard. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(4), 595–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hung, Y.-C., & Schumacher, P. B. (2012). Topicality matters: Position-specific demands on Chinese discourse processing. Neuroscience Letters, 511(2), 59–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jackendoff, R. S. (1997). The architecture of the language faculty. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  21. Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Nieuwland, M. S., & van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1098–1111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nunberg, G. (1979). The non-uniqueness of semantic solutions: Polysemy. Linguistic and Philosophy, 3, 143–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nunberg, G. (1995). Transfers of meaning. Journal of Semantics, 12, 109–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Recanati, F. (2010). Truth-conditional pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Schumacher, P. B. (2009). Definiteness marking shows late effects during discourse processing: Evidence from ERPs. Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence, 5847, 91–106.Google Scholar
  28. Schumacher, P. B. (2011). The hepatitis called…: Electrophysiological evidence for enriched composition. In J. Meibauer & M. Steinbach (Eds.), Experimental pragmatics/semantics (pp. 199–219). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  29. Schumacher, P. B. (2012). Context in neurolinguistics: Time-course data from electrophysiology. In R. Finkbeiner, J. Meibauer, & P. B. Schumacher (Eds.), What is a context? Linguistic approaches and challenges (pp. 33–53). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  30. Schumacher, P. B., & Baumann, S. (2010). Pitch accent type affects the N400 during referential processing. NeuroReport, 21(9), 618–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Schumacher, P. B., & Hung, Y.-C. (2012). Positional influences on information packaging: Insights from topological fields in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(2), 295–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Schumacher, P. B., & Meibauer, J. (2013). Pragmatic inferences and expert knowledge. In F. Liedtke & C. Schulze (Eds.), Beyond words. Content, context, and inference (pp. 231–248). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  33. Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Chambers, C. G., & Carlson, G. N. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71(2), 109–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Stalnaker, R. C. (1999). Context and content. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Streb, J., Rösler, F., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). Event-related responses to pronoun and proper name anaphors in parallel and nonparallel discourse structures. Brain and Language, 70(2), 273–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Swinney, D. A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(6), 645–659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Vallduví, E. (1992). The informational component. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  38. van Berkum, J. J. A., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(6), 657–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. van Berkum, J. J. A., Holleman, B., Nieuwland, M., Otten, M., & Murre, J. (2009). Right or wrong? The brain’s fast response to morally objectionable statements. Psychological Science, 20(9), 1092–1099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. van Berkum, J. J. A., van den Brink, D., Tesink, C. M. J. Y., Kos, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). The neural integration of speaker and message. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 580–591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wang, L., & Schumacher, P. B. (2013). New is not always costly: Evidence form online processing of topic and contrast in Japanese. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(363), 1–20.Google Scholar
  42. Ward, G. (2004). Equatives and deferred reference. Language, 80(2), 262–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Independent Emmy Noether-Research Group, Department of English and LinguisticsJohannes Gutenberg-University MainzMainzGermany
  2. 2.Department of German Language and LiteratureUniversity CologneCologneGermany

Personalised recommendations