Notes
In his Foreword, CIA Deputy Director of Intelligence Douglas MacEachin notes: “I know from first-hand encounters that many CIA officers tend to react skeptically to treatises on analytic epistemology. This is understandable.” (Heuer 1999) I know likewise from first-hand encounters that many analytic epistemologists tend to react skeptically to CIA treatises on intelligence analysis (cf. Gendler and Hawthorne 2005; as well as responses to von Fintel and Gillies 2008). While this too is understandable, it would be misplaced in this particular case.
Though human beings are notoriously bad at making use of explicitly-presented probabilistic base rate information (like the 85:15 information in the airplanes case), we are actually very good at making use of base rate information that is acquired through implicit learning or presented to us in terms of frequencies rather than probabilities. (Indeed, this fact will play a role in the next example.) For detailed discussion of and evidence for this claim, see Koehler (1996). (Thanks to David Hilbert for emphasizing the need to include reference to this body of work, and for pointing out the internal tensions in earlier drafts of the paper that resulted from my not having done so explicitly.).
Readers who enjoyed this brief excursion into CIA publications and who are looking to while (not “wile”; Zwicky 2008) away a few spare hours might enjoy playing some of the Agency’s games for children at https://www.cia.gov/kids-page/games/index.htm. (Central Intelligence Agency 2010) (Readers wondering how I while away mine are invited to follow this paper’s footnotes for a guided tour.)
Actually, in the original quotation, Franklin writes: “It was during our stroll through the club that a white woman called me out, presented me with her coat check, and ordered me to bring her coat” (italics added). I have omitted the crucial term because including it renders race so salient that it would provide an alternative explanation for the response, diluting the force of my tu quoque. (Thanks to Keith DeRose for pointing this out.).
Indeed, as far as I can tell, of the seven authors listed in the first substantive paragraph of the Digital History site’s “succinct essay on the best books on the history of slavery,” none is of African descent. (Digital History n.d.) So if this was your (tacit) reasoning process, you should be credited not with accurate base-rate encoding, but—more likely—with employing what psychologists call the “representativeness heuristic.” (Thanks to Steve Darwall for reminding me that most books on African-American slavery are in fact by non-black authors.)
In case you are fortunate enough not to know the joke, here’s a version from Wikiquote, with George Bernard Shaw (“GBS”) in the leading role. GBS: “Madam, would you sleep with me for a million pounds?” Actress: “My goodness, Well, I'd certainly think about it.” GBS: “Would you sleep with me for a pound?” Actress: “Certainly not! What kind of woman do you think I am?!” GBS: “Madam, we've already established that. Now we are haggling about the price.” (“George Bernard Shaw” 2011).
I am thus trying to cash out in more detail some remarks I made in the closing pages of Gendler (2008b), where I remarked: “[L]iving in a society that violates ones normative ideals has unavoidable cognitive consequences. For either you will need to deliberately restrict your attention or experiences so as not to encode certain sorts of genuine regularities (for example, by deliberately preventing yourself from acquiring and attending to the fact that, in contemporary American society, certain racial categories are associated with certain sorts of highly-valenced affective content.) Or you will need to engage in alief-driven rationalization, changing your normative ideals to accord with the relevant sorts of experienced regularity (for example, by coming to endorse the legitimacy of these stereotypical associations.) Or you will experience the cognitive costs of disharmony, redeploying cognitive energy to suppress the pull of your belief-discordant aliefs (for example, by expending executive control in cases of interracial interaction to suppress your aliefs, thereby temporarily depleting your cognitive resources.) This is the trichotomy of norm-discordant alief.” (Gendler 2008b, p. 578).
Matters here are actually quite complicated, and those familiar with the relevant psychological literature will be aware that I am skating over a large number of important distinctions, both in the base rate literature itself (again, see Kohler 1996 for a review) as well as more specific work on the relation between stereotypes and base-rate neglect (e.g., Locksley et al. 1980; Kunda and Spencer 2003.) Since the goal of this (already overlong) paper is just to explore one aspect of this incredibly complex and multifaceted phenomenon, I beg forgiveness for neglecting these important subtleties.
An important bibliographic caveat. In my discussion below, I will make reference to a wide range of empirical literature in psychology, but almost none to the tremendously important work in philosophy that has addressed these questions. It should be obvious to those familiar with those works that I have been influenced profoundly in my thinking by books such as (Alcoff 2006; Anderson 2010; Appiah and Gutman 1996; Blum 2002; Mills 1997; Shelby 2005; Sullivan 2006; as well as numerous articles by these and other authors.)
In this paper, I focus largely on racial categories in late 20th and early 21st century America. But the basic psychological mechanisms that underlie the general points I am making appear to be universal. See, for example, Hodson et al. (2005) (a study of aversive racism outside the US); Sangrigoli et al. (2005) (a study of cross-race face identification in Europe); Tanaka et al. 2004 (a cross-cultural study of cross-race face identification); and Dunham et al. (2006) (a study exploring implicit bias in America and Japan).
I had originally hoped to include a final section describing promising strategies for overcoming some of the phenomena discussed in this paper, but the essay is already far over its allotted length. I hope to explore these issues in a subsequent paper (especially since eliminating that section required me to leave out some of the paper’s best jokes.) For a philosophically informed overview of some of this literature, see Kelly and Roedder (2008) and Kelly et al. (2010).
Thanks to Matthew Noah Smith for encouraging me to be explicit about this point from the outset.
For interesting related work see (Huebner 2009).
The contemporary term stereotype was coined by Lippmann in (1922). He writes: “For the most part we do not first see, and then define; we define first and then see. In the great blooming, buzzing confusion of the outer world we pick out what our culture has already defined for us, and we tend to perceive that which we have picked out in the form stereotyped for us by our culture." (Lippmann 1922, p. 81). In keeping with the dual themes of the opening section of this essay, it’s worth noting that Lippmann is also the person who introduced the term Cold War into popular discourse (Lippmann 1947), following its use in the context of US-Soviet relations by Bernard Baruch, an advisor to Harry Truman, in speech before Congress on April 14, 1947. (Baruch 1947).
The entry continues: “Functionally, they may be regarded as mental helpers that operate in the form of heuristics or short-cuts. Historically, this view of stereotypes as devices that allow a sensible reading of a complex world marked a breakthrough in research on stereotypes. Advanced by Allport (1954) and Tajfel (1969), the idea that stereotypes were inherent to the act of social categorization now forms the basis of the modern view. As such, stereotypes are regarded to be ordinary in nature, in the sense that they are the byproducts of basic processes of perception and categorization, learning, and memory. This cognitive view of stereotypes has dominated the field since the early 1980s” (Banaji 2002).
In the seminal study exploring this effect, Tajfel and Wilkes (1963) asked participants to estimate category exemplars (lines) that varied continuously along a physical dimension (length). Participants provided their estimates in one of three conditions: Longer lines were systematically given a different label from shorter lines (with an arbitrary point along the continuum selected for the division), each line was randomly given one of the two labels, or no labels were presented. As predicted, the systematic association of categorical labels with exemplars that varied along a physical continuum increased the perceived differences between exemplars from different categories: Participants perceived a greater difference between the shortest of the long lines and the longest of the short lines in the systematic-categorization condition than they did in the other two conditions (thereby providing support for Tajfel and Wilkes’ hypothesis that some aspects of stereotyping have their origins in cognitive-perceptual processes that lead to an exaggeration of perceived differences between members of different social groups). In subsequent research in this tradition, Tajfel and others have demonstrated this effect in a wide range of domains (cf. Tajfel 1959; see also Ford and Stangor 1992; Krueger and Clement 1994; Macrae et al. 1993; Macrae et al. 1994; Stangor 2009; Tajfel 1981; Tajfel and Wilkes 1963).
Cf. Patricia Devine: “[D]uring socialization, a culture’s beliefs about various social groups are frequently activated and become well-learned. As a result, these deep-rooted stereotypes and evaluative biases are automatically activated, without conscious awareness or intention, in the presence of members of stereotyped groups (or their symbolic equivalent)” (Devine and Sharp 2009, p. 62; cf. Devine 1989).
Thanks to George Bealer for encouraging me to include more detail on this issue.
I present the skywalk case making use of a strict characterization of alief, according to which the alief’s content is a representational-affective-behavioral triad. But the term fits more easily into ordinary talk if it is given a propositional complement. When the associative chain is widely shared, little information is lost thereby. So I might say, speaking loosely: I believe that the walkway is solid but alieve that it is precarious.
Literally on the day that this paper was going to press, New York Times reporter Edward Rothstein (author of the Grand Canyon Skywalk article cited at the beginning of “Alief and Belief”) published a piece about Chicago’s Skydeck Ledge, a transparent glass box suspended on the side of the 103-story Willis Tower, some 1,353 feet above the city’s busy streets. In perfect keeping with the alief-belief theme, Rothstein reports: “Despite the reassuring rivets in the 1,500-pound glass panels, the calm stillness of the air at the Windy City’s pinnacle and the security of a 10,000-pound weight capacity for each of the four 4.3-foot-deep glass boxes that protrude past the sheer edge of the Western Hemisphere’s tallest building—despite all that, you still feel twinges of queasiness…It is comforting to know that they were designed by the building’s original architects, Skidmore, Owings and Merrill, and mounted on tracks that allow them to be pulled inside for cleaning and maintenance. But any reassurance is undercut by the elimination of nearly all visible support; you walk within them, 1,353 feet above South Wacker Drive, surrounded by open air and unbounded space.” (The article continues with some interesting observations about why the effect of the Ledge is more powerful than that of the Canyon Skywalk; see Rothstein (2011)).
Another domain where alief-belief discordance may be valuable is in the horror that most humans confront in undertaking necessary violence. Even if a person believes it is necessary to harm or kill for the greater good, we think they have lost something fundamental to their humanity if they lose or suppress the ‘killing-is-bad’ alief. (Thanks to Brendan Dill for discussion here.)
As Patricia Devine writes: “during socialization, a culture’s beliefs about various social groups are frequently activated and become well-learned. As a result, these deep-rooted stereotypes and evaluative biases are automatically activated, without conscious awareness or intention, in the presence of members of stereotyped groups (or their symbolic equivalent)” (Devine 2009, p. 62; cf. Devine 1989).
Nor is this tension a new one. In 1944, Gunnar Myrdal wrote of “the ever-raging conflict between, on the one hand, the valuations preserved on the general plane which we call the ‘American creed,’ where the American thinks, talks, and acts under the influence of high national and Christian precepts and, on the other hand, the valuations on the specific planes of individual and group living, where personal and local interests; economic, social, and sexual jealousies; consideration of community prestige and conformity; group prejudice against particular persons or types of people; and all sorts of miscellaneous wants, impulses, and habits dominate his outlook. (Myrdal 1944, p. xliii, as cited in Gaertner and Dovidio 2000).
Cf. also Devine and Elliot: “The coexistence of a rejected yet enduring negative stereotype of Blacks and a positive set of beliefs about Blacks places the low-prejudiced social perceiver in a precarious position. A considerable amount of research indicates that stereotypes can be automatically activated by the perception of social stimuli (e.g. exemplars of the group, group labels), resulting in prejudice-like feelings, thoughts and behaviors for high- and low-prejudiced individuals alike (Devine 1989; Klinger and Beall 1992).” (Devine and Elliott 1995, p. 1147).
Similar patterns were observed in a 1986 meta-analysis of 128 eyewitness identification and facial recognition studies, involving 960 experimental conditions and 16,950 participants (Shapiro and Penrod 1986).
This is an important caveat, and one that brings out the ways in which I am ignoring in my discussions a large and important literature on racial categorization itself.
To my knowledge, there have been no empirical studies in this paradigm looking at black responses to white faces. One might expect parallel results (mutatis mutandis). Or one might expect that because white identity is encoded as the unmarked case in the dominant culture, the effect would be less pronounced.
Thanks to Christopher Peacocke for the suggestion that prompted this analogy.
Thanks to Brendan Dill for pointing out the importance of making this distinction, and for suggesting the second formulation.
For an interesting discussion of the interaction of race and class in CR deficit, see Shriver et al. (2008).
The magnitude of these effects can be quite powerful: in some cases, participants in stereotype threat conditions answer only half as many questions correctly as those in non-threat conditions (Steele and Aronson 1995).
Interestingly, the effect seems to be most profound for challenging rather than simple problems. Neuville and Croizet (2007) found, for example, that activating gender identity increased girls’ performance on easy math problems, but that this advantage was outweighed by a large decrease in their performance on more difficult problems.
For a stereotype-threat-avoidance strategy that exploits this phenomenon, see Rydell et al. (2009).
As with any complex social phenomenon, there are undoubtedly a range of factors that contribute to the effect. Theories to explain the phenomenon (or, more likely, phenomena) are manifold, and a full survey would require a full paper. For a representative example of such a theory, cf. Schmader et al. (2008), who hypothesize that “stereotype threat disrupts performance via 3 distinct, yet interrelated, mechanisms: (a) a physiological stress response that directly impairs prefrontal processing, (b) a tendency to actively monitor performance, and (c) efforts to suppress negative thoughts and emotions in the service of self-regulation. These mechanisms combine to consume executive resources needed to perform well on cognitive and social tasks. The active monitoring mechanism disrupts performance on sensorimotor tasks directly.” (Schmader et al. 2008, p. 336).
I don’t know of empirical work that tries to tease apart these two possibilities.
Cf. YouTube’s “Drunk History” videos, e.g. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ipV2u-MxlFc (see Drunk History n.d.) (Thanks to Brendan Dill for this pointer).
Relatedly, extant hypotheses that give rise to confirmation biases can be seen as operating via easons rather than reasons. (Thanks to Brendan Dill for discussion on this point.)
Some of the discussion in this section follows that in the closing pages of Gendler (2008b).
Given the argument I am making, it does not matter whether the implicit associations revealed by the IAT measure the subject’s ease of access to associations that she implicitly (though perhaps not explicitly) endorses or whether it measures her ease of access to culturally-encoded associations that she both implicitly and explicitly rejects. For discussion of this controversy, see (Karpinski and Hilton 2001; Olson and Fazio 2004; Arkes and Tetlock 2004).
For connection of this to historical discussions of the “harmonious soul” see Gendler (2008b).
Heretical counterfactuals are essentially cases that invoke what I call “imaginative resistance” (Gendler 2000, 2006). So, for example, Tetlock et al. presented participants with cases such as the following: “Consider the argument If Joseph had not believed the message that Mary had conceived a child through the Holy Ghost and that there was no reason to fear taking Mary as his wife, then Jesus would have grown up without the influence of a father and would have formed a very different personality. Is it easy or difficult to accept the premise that Joseph could have decided not to believe the angel's message?” (Tetlock et al. 2000, p. 864.) I offer an alief-involving explanation of imaginative resistance in Gendler (2000, 2006, 2010).
LaFree et al. (2010) also includes a discussion of effectiveness of various ameliorative strategies.
References
Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V., et al. (2006). They all look the same to me (unless they’re angry): From out-group homogeneity to out-group heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17(10), 836–840.
Alcoff, L. M. (2006). Visible identities: Race, gender, and the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 12(5), 385–390.
Amodio, D. M., & Devine, P. G. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 652–661.
Anderson, E. (2010). The imperative of integration. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Appiah, K. A., & Gutman, A. (1996). Color conscious: The political morality of race. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or “Would Jesse Jackson ‘fail’ the Implicit Association Test?”. Psychological Inquiry, 15(4), 257–278.
Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M., & Browin, J. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35(1), 29–46.
Ayres, I., Vars, F. E., Zakariya, N. (2004–2005). To insure prejudice: Racial disparities in taxicab tipping. Yale Law Journal, 114, 1613.
Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., & Melartin, R. L. (1993). Infants’ ability to draw inferences about nonobvious object properties: Evidence from exploratory play. Child Development, 64(3), 711–728.
Banaji, M. R. (2002). Stereotypes, social psychology of. In N. Smelser & P. Baltes (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 15100–15104). New York, NY: Pergamon.
Bar-Haim, Y., Ziv, T., Lamy, D., & Hodes, R. M. (2006). Nature and nurture in own-race face processing. Psychological Science, 17(2), 159–163.
Baruch, B. M. (1947). More production for peace. Vital Speeches of the Day, 13(14), 425.
Beilock, S. L., Jellison, W. A., Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Carr, T. H. (2006). On the causal mechanisms of stereotype threat: Can skills that don’t rely heavily on working memory still be threatened? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 1059–1071.
Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., & Hugenberg, K. (2007). The cross-race category effect: Mere social categorization is sufficient to elicit an own-group bias in face recognition. Psychological Science, 18(8), 706–712.
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. The American Economic Review, 94(4), 991–1013.
Blum, L. (2002). “I’m not a racist, but”.: The moral quandary of race. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Bosson, J. K., Haymovitz, E. L., & Pinel, E. C. (2004). When saying and doing diverge: The effects of stereotype threat on self-reported versus non-verbal anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(2), 247–255.
Butler, D. M., & Broockman, D. E. (forthcoming). Do politicians racially discriminate against constituents? A field experiment on state legislators. American Journal of Political Science.
Central Intelligence Agency. (2010). Games. https://www.cia.gov/kids-page/games/index.html. Accessed 28 April.
Chudler, E. H. (n.d.). Colors, colors. http://faculty.washington.edu/chudler/words.html. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1314–1329.
Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Kurzban, B. (2003). Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 173–179.
Croizet, J.-C., & Claire, T. (1998). Extending the concept of stereotype threat to social class: The intellectual underperformance of students from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 588–594.
Croizet, J.-C., Desprès, G., Gauzins, M.-E., Huguet, P., Leyens, J.-P., & Méot, A. (2004). Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(6), 721–731.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5–18.
Devine, P. G., & Elliott, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1139–1150.
Devine, P. G., & Sharp, L. B. (2009). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotyping and prejudice. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 61–82). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Digital History. (n.d.). http://www.digitalhistory.uh.edu/modules/slavery/bibliographical_essay.html. Accessed 21 May 2011.
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Aversive racism. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology: (Vol. 36, pp. 1–52). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, K. (in press). Racism. In J. F. Dovidio, M. Hewstone, P. Glick, & V. M. Esses (Eds.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Drunk History. (n.d.). http://www.hbo.com/funny-or-die-presents/about/video/drunk-history.html. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city to Japanese village: A cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child Development, 77(5), 1268–1281.
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). Children and social groups: A developmental analysis of implicit consistency in Hispanic Americans. Self and Identity, 6(2), 238–255.
Farmer, A., & Terrell, D. (2001). Crime versus justice: Is there a trade-off? Journal of Law and Economics, 44(2), 345–366.
Ford, T. E., & Stangor, C. (1992). The role of diagnosticity in stereotype formation: Perceiving group means and variances. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 356–367.
Franklin, J. H. (2005). Mirror to America: The autobiography of John Hope Franklin. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Gendler, T. S. (2000). The puzzle of imaginative resistance. The Journal of Philosophy, 97(2), 55–81.
Gendler, T. S. (2006). Imaginative resistance revisited. In S. Nichols (Ed.), The architecture of the imagination (pp. 149–174). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gendler, T. S. (2008a). Alief and belief. Journal of Philosophy, 105(10), 634–663.
Gendler, T. S. (2008b). Alief in action (and reaction). Mind & Language, 23(5), 552–585.
Gendler, T. S. (2010). Intuition, imagination and philosophical methodology: Selected papers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gendler, T. S., & Hawthorne, J. (2005). The real guide to fake barns: A catalogue of gifts for your epistemic enemies. Philosophical Studies, 124(3), 331–352.
George Bernard Shaw. (n.d.). In Wikiquote. http://en.wikiquote.org/wiki/George_Bernard_Shaw#Disputed. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Gonzalez, P. M., Blanton, H., & Williams, K. J. (2002). The effects of stereotype threat and double-minority status on the test performance of Latino women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(5), 659–670.
Graham, S. A., Kilbreath, C. S., & Welder, A. N. (2001). Words and shape similarity guide 13- month-olds’ inferences about nonobvious object properties. In J. D. Moore & K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-third annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 352–357). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, J. W. (1994). Stereotypes. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hamilton, D. L., & Trolier, T. K. (1986). Stereotypes and stereotyping: An overview of the cognitive approach. In J. Dovidio & S. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 127–163). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Hehman, E., Mania, E. W., & Gaertner, S. L. (2010). Where the division lies: Common ingroup identity moderates the cross-race effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 445–448.
Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S. J., & Rahhal, T. A. (2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age differences in memory performance. Journal of Gerontology, 58(1), 3–11.
Heuer, R. J. (1999). Psychology of intelligence analysis. Washington, DC: Center for Study of Intelligence, Central Intelligence Agency.
Hills, P. J., & Lewis, M. B. (2006). Reducing the own-race bias in face recognition by shifting attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 996–1002.
Hodson, G., Hooper, H., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2005). Aversive racism in Britain: Legal decisions and the use of inadmissible evidence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(4), 437–448.
Huebner, B. (2009). Troubles with stereotypes for spinozan minds. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 39(1), 63–92.
Hugenberg, K., Miller, J., & Claypool, H. M. (2007). Categorization and individuation in the cross- race recognition deficit: Toward a solution to an insidious problem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 334–340.
IAT Corp. (1998). Project implicit. https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/. Accessed 31 May 2011.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Co.
Jesse Jackson. (n.d.). In Wikiquote. http://en.wikiquote.org/wiki/Jesse_Jackson. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). On prediction and judgment. Oregon Research Institute Research Bulletin, 12(4).
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80(4), 237–251.
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 774–788.
Kelly, D. R., Machery, E., & Mallon, R. (2010). Race and racial cognition. In J. M. Doris, et al. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of moral psychology (pp. 433–472). Oxford University Press: Oxford.
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1084–1089.
Kelly, D. R., & Roedder, E. (2008). Racial cognition and the ethics of implicit bias. Philosophy Compass, 3(3), 522–540.
Klinger, M., & Beall, P. (1992, May). Conscious and unconscious effects of stereotype activation. Paper Presented at the 64th Annual Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.
Koehler, J. J. (1996). The base rate fallacy reconsidered: Normative, descriptive, and methodological challenges. Behavioral & Brain Science, 19(1), 1–17.
Kovel, J. (1970). White racism: A psychohistory. New York, NY: Pantheon Books, Random House.
Krueger, J., & Clement, R. W. (1994). The truly false consensus effect: An ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 596–610.
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 522–544.
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(26), 15387–15392.
LaFree, G., Baumer, E. P., & O’Brien, R. (2010). Still separate and unequal? A city-level analysis of the black-white gap in homicide arrests since 1960. American Sociological Review, 75(1), 75–100.
Lebrecht, S., Pierce, L. J., Tarr, M. J., & Tanaka, J. W. (2009). Perceptual other-race training reduces implicit racial bias. PLoS ONE, 4(1), e4215.
Leslie, S. J. (forthcoming). The original sin of cognition: Fear, prejudice, and generalization. The Journal of Philosophy.
Lever, A. (2005). Why racial profiling is hard to justify: A response to Risse and Zeckhauser. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 33(1), 94–110.
Lippman, W. (1922). Public opinion. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace.
Lippman, W. (1947). The cold war: a study in US foreign policy. New York, NY: Harper.
Locksley, A., Borgida, E., Brekke, N., & Hepburn, C. (1980). Sex stereotypes and social judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 821–831.
Macrae, C. N., Hewstone, M., & Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Processing load and memory for stereotype-based information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23(1), 77–87.
Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(1), 37–47.
McGugin, R. W., Tanaka, J. W., Lebrecht, S., Tarr, M. J., & Gauthier, I. (2011). Race-specific perceptual discrimination improvement following short individuation training with faces. Cognitive Science, 35(2), 330–347.
Medin, D. L. (1988). Social categorization: Structures, processes, and purposes. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition: (Vol. 1, pp. 119–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 3–35.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 343–355.
Mills, C. W. (1997). The racial contract. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Myrdal, G. (1944). An American dilemma: The Negro problem and the modern democracy. New York, NY: Harper & Bros.
Neuville, E., & Croizet, J. C. (2007). Can salience of gender identity impair math performance among 7–8 years old girls? The moderating role of task difficulty. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22(3), 307–316.
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The implicit association test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 265–292). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Oakes, P. J., & Turner, J. C. (1990). Is limited information processing capacity the cause of social stereotyping? In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 111–135). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the implicit association test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 653–667.
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(2), 181–192.
Payne, B. K. (2006). Weapon bias: Split-second decisions and unintended stereotyping. Current directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 287–291.
Payne, B. K., Lambert, A. J., & Jacoby, L. L. (2002). Best laid plans: Effects of goals on accessibility bias and cognitive control in racebased misperceptions of weapons. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(4), 384–396.
Pious, S. (2002). Implicit association test. http://www.understandingprejudice.org/iat/. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat on women’s generation of mathematical problem solving strategies. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 55–71.
Richeson, J. A., Baird, A. A., Gordon, H. L., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L., Trawalter, S., et al. (2003). An fMRI examination of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nature Neuroscience, 6(12), 1323–1328.
Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). When prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychological Science, 14(3), 287–290.
Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Negotiating interracial interactions: Costs, consequences, and possibilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 316–320.
Richeson, J. A., Trawalter, S., & Shelton, J. N. (2005). African-Americans’ racial attitudes and the depletion of executive function after interracial interactions. Social Cognition, 23(4), 336–352.
Risse, M., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Racial profiling. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 32(2), 131–170.
Rothstein, E. (2011). Suspended in space, 103 stories over Chicago. http://www.nytimes.com/2011/05/31/arts/design/willis-tower-suspends-visitors-above-chicago.html?_r=2&scp=2&sq=chicago&st=cse. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Multiple social identities and stereotype threat: Imbalance, accessibility, and working memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 949–966.
Sangrigoli, S., Pallier, C., Argenti, A.-M., Ventureyra, V. A. G., & de Schonen, S. (2005). Reversibility of the other-race effect in face recognition during childhood. Psychological Science, 16(6), 440–444.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2), 336–356.
Schneider, D. J. (2004). The psychology of stereotyping. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Shapiro, P. N., & Penrod, S. (1986). Meta-analysis of facial identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 100(2), 139–156.
Shelby, T. (2005). We who are dark: The philosophical foundations of black solidarity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Shih, M., Pittinsky, T. L., & Trahan, A. (2006). Domain-specific effects of stereotypes on performance. Self and Identity, 5(1), 1–14.
Shriver, E. R., Young, S. G., Hugenberg, K., Bernstein, M. J., & Lantner, J. R. (2008). Class, race, and the face: Social context modulates the cross-race effect in face recognition. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(2), 260–274.
Sporer, S. L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 36–97.
Stangor, C. (2009). The study of stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination within social psychology: A quick history of theory and research. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 1–22). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Steele, C. M. (2010). Whistling Vivaldi: And other clues to how stereotypes affect us (issues of our time). New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Co.
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797–811.
Stone, J. (2002). Battling doubt by avoiding practice: The effects of stereotype threat on self- handicapping in white athletes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1667–1678.
Stone, J., Lynch, C. I., Sjomeling, M., & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects on black and white athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1213–1227.
Stroessner, S., Good, C., & Webster, L. (n.d.). http://reducingstereotypethreat.org. Accessed 21 May 2011.
Sullivan, S. (2006). Revealing whiteness: The unconscious habits of racial privilege. Bloomington and Indianapolis, IN: University of Indiana Press.
Tajfel, H. (1959). Quantitative judgment in social perception. British Journal of Psychology, 50(1), 16–29.
Tajfel, H. (1969). Cognitive aspects of prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 25(4), 79–97.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., & Wilkes, A. L. (1963). Classification and quantitative judgment. British Journal of Psychology, 54(2), 101–114.
Tanaka, J. W., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C. M. (2004). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: Evidence from a cross cultural study. Cognition, 93(1), B1–B9.
Tanaka, J. W., & Piece, L. J. (2009). The neural plasticity of other-race face recognition. Cognitive Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(1), 122–131.
Taylor, S. E. (1981). A categorization approach to stereotyping. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 145–182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tetlock, P. F., Kristel, O., Elson, B., Green, M., & Lerner, J. (2000). The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 853–870.
Trawalter, S., & Richeson, J. A. (2006). Regulatory focus and executive function after interracial interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(3), 406–412.
Trawalter, S., & Richeson, J. A. (2008). Let’s talk about race, baby! When whites’ and blacks’ interracial contact experiences diverge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1214–1217.
Trawalter, S., Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2009). Predicting behavior during interracial interactions: A stress and coping approach. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(4), 243–268.
von Fintel, K., & Gillies, A. (2008). CIA leaks. Philosophical Review, 117(1), 77–98.
WebMD. (2010). Why breakfast is the most important meal of the day. http://www.webmd.com/diet/guide/most-important-meal. Accessed 27 May 2011.
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 277–295.
Wraga, M., Duncan, L., Jacobs, E. C., Helt, M., & Church, J. (2006). Stereotype susceptibility narrows the gender gap in imagined self-rotation performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 813–819.
Zwicky, A. (2008). Wile away. http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=466. Accessed 31 May 2011.
Acknowledgements
For comments and conversation regarding early drafts, I am grateful to Richard Brooks, Jack Dovidio, Andy Egan, Jeffrey Fagan, David Hilbert, Richard Holton, Joshua Knobe, Tony Laden, Alex Madva, Jennifer Nagel, Aaron Norby, Jonathan Phillips, Richard Samuels, Eric Schwitzgebel, Susanna Siegel, Matthew Noah Smith and J.D. Trout. Thanks also to the members of John Bargh’s and Jack Dovidio’s labs (Fall 2009–Spring 2010), who introduced me to the bodies of psychological literature to which this paper makes appeal. I am immensely grateful to the audiences to whom I had a chance to present this material, including those at the Oberlin Colloquium in Epistemology (April 2010) and the Harvard/MIT “Belief and its Cousins” Mini-conference (January 2011), and at the Departments of Philosophy at Brown University (October 2010), Columbia University (February 2011), Northwestern University (March 2011), University of Illinois at Chicago Circle (March 2011) and the University of Pittsburgh (April 2011); I regret that I did not take notes in ways that allow me to acknowledge specific contributions in those settings, but they are manifold. Special thanks to my Yale Philosophy colleagues—including Facundo Alonso, George Bealer, Steve Darwall, Jay Elliot, Verity Harte, Shelly Kagan, David Possen, Barbara Sattler and Zoltán Gendler Szabó—for an incredibly helpful Faculty Lunch discussion in April 2011, and to Brendan Dill for a careful reading of the paper’s penultimate draft. My greatest thanks are reserved for my extraordinary research assistant, Takuya Sawaoka, for his tireless bibliographic work and numerous outstanding suggestions, both stylistic and substantive.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Gendler, T.S. On the epistemic costs of implicit bias. Philos Stud 156, 33–63 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-011-9801-7
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-011-9801-7