Advertisement

Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences

, Volume 17, Issue 3, pp 455–484 | Cite as

The bored mind is a guiding mind: toward a regulatory theory of boredom

  • Andreas ElpidorouEmail author
Article

Abstract

By presenting and synthesizing findings on the character of boredom, the article advances a theoretical account of the function of the state of boredom. The article argues that the state of boredom should be understood as a functional emotion that is both informative and regulatory of one's behavior. Boredom informs one of the presence of an unsatisfactory situation and, at the same time, it motivates one to pursue a new goal when the current goal ceases to be satisfactory, attractive or meaningful. Boredom ultimately promotes both movement and the restoration of the perception that one's activities are meaningful and congruent with one's overall projects.

Keywords

Boredom Emotions Affectivity Self-regulation Movement Interest Function 

References

  1. Acee, T. W., Kim, H., Kim, H. J., Kim, J., Hsiang-Ning, R. C., Kim, M., Cho, Y., & Wicker, F. W. (2010). Academic boredom in under-and over-challenging situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(1), 17–27.Google Scholar
  2. Ahmed, S. M. S. (1990). Psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 963–966.Google Scholar
  3. Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  4. Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  5. Anderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 1–9.Google Scholar
  6. Anderson, J. S. (2007). Pleasant, pleasurable, and positive activities. In L. L’Abate (Ed.), Low-cost approaches to promote physical and mental health. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  7. Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 972–982.Google Scholar
  8. Avni-Babad, D., & Ritov, I. (2003). Routine and the perception of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 132, 543–555.Google Scholar
  9. Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 614–636.Google Scholar
  10. Barbalet, J. M. (1999). Boredom and social meaning. The British Journal of Sociology, 50(4), 631–646.Google Scholar
  11. Barmack, J. E. (1937). Boredom and other factors in the physiology of mental effort: An exploratory study. Archives of Psychology, 31, 9–10.Google Scholar
  12. Barwick, F., Arnett, P., & Slobounov, S. (2012). EEG correlates of fatigue during administration of a neuropsychological test battery. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(2), 278–284.Google Scholar
  13. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265.Google Scholar
  14. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323.Google Scholar
  15. Belton, T., & Priyadharshini, E. (2007). Boredom and schooling: A cross-disciplinary exploration. Cambridge Journal of Education, 37(4), 579–595.Google Scholar
  16. Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2013). On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences, 3(3), 459–472.Google Scholar
  17. Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  18. Berlyne, D. E. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. In A. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln.Google Scholar
  19. Bernstein, H. E. (1975). Boredom and the ready-made life. Social Research, 42, 512–537.Google Scholar
  20. Binnema, D. (2004). Interrelations of psychiatric patient experiences of boredom and mental health. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25, 833–842.Google Scholar
  21. Blaszczynski, A., McConaghy, N., & Frankova, A. (1990). Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 67(1), 35–42.Google Scholar
  22. Block, R. A., & Reed, M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 656–665.Google Scholar
  23. Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(2), 184–197.Google Scholar
  24. Braby, C. D., Harris, D., & Muir, H. C. (1993). A psychophysiological approach to the assessment of work underload. Ergonomics, 36(9), 1035–1042.Google Scholar
  25. Bull, P. E. (1987). Posture and Gesture (International series in experimental social psychology, volume 16). Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  26. Chabot, R. J., & Serfontein, G. (1996). Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40(10), 951–963.Google Scholar
  27. Chanel, G., Rebetez, C., Bétrancourt, M., & Pun, T. (2008, October). Boredom, engagement and anxiety as indicators for adaptation to difficulty in games. Proceedings of MindTrek ‘08, pp. 13-17. Tampere, Finland. doi: 10.1145/1457199.1457203.
  28. Chaplin, W. F., John, O. P., & Goldberg, L. R. (1988). Conceptions of states and traits: Dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 541–557.Google Scholar
  29. Charlton, J., & Hertz, R. (1989). Guarding against boredom. Security specialists in the US Air Force. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 18, 299–326.Google Scholar
  30. Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: Experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–368. doi: 10.1037/emo0000232.Google Scholar
  31. Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Vol. 423, pp. 566–582). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  32. Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (1998). EEG analysis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Research, 81(1), 19–29.Google Scholar
  33. Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 3 2 3–417). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  34. Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., & Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361.Google Scholar
  35. Cordes, S., Williams, C. L., & Meck, W. H. (2007). Common representations of abstract quantities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 156–161.Google Scholar
  36. Costa, P. T, McCrae, R. R., & Arenberg, D. (1980). Enduring dispositions in adult males. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 793–800.Google Scholar
  37. Craig, S. D., D'Mello, S., Witherspoon, A., & Graesser, A. (2008). Emote aloud during learning with AutoTutor: Applying the facial action coding system to cognitive–affective states during learning. Cognition and Emotion, 22(5), 777–788.Google Scholar
  38. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  39. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
  40. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self: A psychology for the third millennium. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  41. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Cummings, M. L., Gao, F., & Thornburg, K. M. (2016). Boredom in the workplace a new look at an old problem. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 58(2), 279–300.Google Scholar
  43. Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., & Kuhlman, M. M. (2005). Driving anger, sensation seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the prediction of unsafe driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37(2), 341–348.Google Scholar
  44. Damrad-Frye, R., & Laird, J. D. (1989). The experience of boredom: The role of the self-perception of attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 315–320.Google Scholar
  45. Danckert, J. (2013). Descent of the doldrums. Scientific American Mind, 24(3), 54–59.Google Scholar
  46. Danckert, J. A., & Allman, A. A. A. (2005). Time flies when you’re having fun: Temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain and Cognition, 59(3), 236–245.Google Scholar
  47. Danckert, J., & Merrifield, C. (2016). Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Experimental Brain Research, 1–12.Google Scholar
  48. Daniels, L. M., Tze, V. M., & Goetz, T. (2015). Examining boredom: Different causes for different coping profiles. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 255–261.Google Scholar
  49. Daschmann, E. C., Goetz, T., & Stupnisky, R. H. (2011). Testing the predictors of boredom at school: Development and validation of the precursors to boredom scales. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 421–440.Google Scholar
  50. De Chenne, T. K. (1988). Boredom as a clinical issue. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 25, 71–81.Google Scholar
  51. De Chenne, T. K., & Moody, A. J. (1988). Boredom: Theory and therapy. The Psychotherapy Patient, 3(3–4), 17–29.Google Scholar
  52. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  53. Drory, A. (1982). Boredom proneness---the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17.Google Scholar
  54. Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., & Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: Boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1035–1045.Google Scholar
  55. Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495.Google Scholar
  56. Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–343). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  57. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial action coding system: Investigator’s guide. Palo Altom: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  58. Elpidorou, A. (2014). The bright side of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1245.Google Scholar
  59. Elpidorou, A. (2015). The quiet alarm. Aeon Magazine. https://aeon.co/essays/life-without-boredom-would-be-a-nightmare.
  60. Elpidorou, A. (2016). The significance of boredom: A Sartrean reading. In. D. Dahlstrom, A. Elpidorou, & W. Hopp, Philosophy of mind and phenomenology: Conceptual and empirical approaches (pp. 268–283). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  61. Elpidorou, A. (2017). The moral dimensions of boredom: A call for research. Review of General Psychology, 21(1), 30–48.Google Scholar
  62. Elpidorou, A. (in press). The good of boredom. Philosophical Psychology.Google Scholar
  63. Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people most of the time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097–1126.Google Scholar
  64. Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield: Thomas.Google Scholar
  65. Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., & Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(3), 307–340.Google Scholar
  66. Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment, 20, 68–85.Google Scholar
  67. Fan, X., Zhou, Q., Liu, Z., & Xie, F. (2015). Electroencephalogram assessment of mental fatigue in visual search. Bio-medical Materials and Engineering, 26(s1), 1455–1463.Google Scholar
  68. Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness--the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(1), 4–17.Google Scholar
  69. Fenichel, O. (1951). On the psychology of boredom. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought: Selected sources (pp. 349–361). New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations, 46, 395–417.Google Scholar
  71. Fiske, D. W. (1961). Effects of monotonous and restricted stimulation. In D. W. Fiske & S. R. Maddi (Eds.), Functions of varied experience (pp. 106–145). Homewood: Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
  72. Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. (1961). Functions of varied experience. Homewood: Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
  73. Flaherty, M. G., Freidin, B., & Sautu, R. (2005). Variation in the perceived passage of time: A cross-national study. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 400–410.Google Scholar
  74. Fleeson, W., Malanos, A. B., & Achille, N. M. (2002). An intraindividual process approach to the relationship between extraversion and positive affect: is acting extraverted as "good" as being extraverted? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1409–1422.Google Scholar
  75. Fraisse, P. (1963). The psychology of time (J. Leith, Trans. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  76. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319.Google Scholar
  77. Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 53.Google Scholar
  78. Fredrickson, B. L., & Cohn, M. A. (2008). Positive emotions. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 777–796) (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  79. Fridhandler, B. M. (1986). Conceptual note on state, trait, and the state–trait distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 169–174.Google Scholar
  80. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  81. Gabriel, M. (1988). Boredom: Exploration of a developmental perspective. Clinical Social Work Journal, 16, 156–164.Google Scholar
  82. Game, A. M. (2007). Workplace boredom coping: Health, safety, and HR implications. Personnel Review, 36, 701–721.Google Scholar
  83. Gana, K., & Akremi, M. (1998). L'échelle de Disposition à l'Ennui (EDE): Adaptation française et validation du Boredom Proneness Scale (BP).L'année. Psychologique, 98(3), 429–450.Google Scholar
  84. Gasper, K., & Middlewood, B. L. (2014). Approaching novel thoughts: Understanding why elation and boredom promote associative thought more than distress and relaxation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 50–57.Google Scholar
  85. Geiwitz, P. J. (1966). Structure of boredom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 592–600.Google Scholar
  86. Giakoumis, D., Vogiannou, A., Kosunen, I., Moustakas, K., Tzovaras, D., & Hassapis, G. (2010) Identifying psychophysiological correlates of boredom and negative mood induced during HCI. BIOSTEC 2010, Valencia, Spain, pp. 3–12.Google Scholar
  87. Gibbon, J., Church, R., & Meck, W. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.Google Scholar
  88. Goetz, T., & Frenzel, A. C. (2006). Phänomenologie schulischer Langeweile. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 38(4), 149–153.Google Scholar
  89. Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., Hall, N., & Haag, L. (2006). Academic emotions from a social-cognitive perspective: Antecedents and domain specificity of students' affect in the context of Latin instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 289–308.Google Scholar
  90. Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., & Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38(3), 401–419.Google Scholar
  91. Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., & Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(6), 647.Google Scholar
  92. Gordon, A., Wilkinson, R., McGown, A., & Jovanoska, A. (1997). The psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale: An examination of its validity. Psychological Studies, 42, 85–97.Google Scholar
  93. Grassian, S. (2006). Psychiatric effects of solitary confinement. Wash. UJL & Pol'y, 22, 325.Google Scholar
  94. Greenson, R. R. (1953). On boredom. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 7–21.Google Scholar
  95. Grondin, S., & Macar, F. (1992). Dividing attention between temporal and nontemporal tasks: A performance operating characteristics—POC—Analysis. In F. Macar, V. Pouthas, & W. J. Friedman (Eds.), Towards bridging the gap (pp. 119–128). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  96. Grubb, E. A. (1975). Assembly line boredom and individual differences in recreation participation. Journal of Leisure Research, 7(4), 256–269.Google Scholar
  97. Hamilton, J. A. (1981). Attention, personality, and the self-regulation of mood: Absorbing interest and boredom. Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 10, 281315.Google Scholar
  98. Hamilton, J. A., Haier, R. J., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1984). Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential, and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 183–193.Google Scholar
  99. Hansen, J., & Trope, Y. (2013). When time flies: How abstract and concrete mental construal affect the perception of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 336–347.Google Scholar
  100. Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 576–598.Google Scholar
  101. Hartocollis, P. (1972). Time as a dimension of affects. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 20, 92–108.Google Scholar
  102. Hasselton, M. G., & Ketelaar, T. (2006). Irrational emotions or emotional wisdom? The evolutionary psychology of emotions and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 21–39). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  103. Havermans, R. C., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos, A., & Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and inflicting pain out of boredom. Appetite, 85, 52–57.Google Scholar
  104. Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the CNS (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 62, 243–254.Google Scholar
  105. Heidegger, M. (1983). Die Grundbegriffe der Metaphysik. Welt-Endlichkeit-Einsamkeit. Frankfurt am Main: Vittorio Klostermann, (The Fundamental Concepts of Metaphysics: World, finitude, solitude. Indiana University Press, 2001).Google Scholar
  106. Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 111–127.Google Scholar
  107. Hill, A. B., & Perkins, R. E. (1985). Towards a model of boredom. British Journal of Psychology, 76(2), 235-240.Google Scholar
  108. Hitchcock, E. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S., Moroney, B. W., & See, J. E. (1999). Effects of cueing and knowledge of results on workload and boredom in sustained attention. Human Factors, 41, 365–372.Google Scholar
  109. Hunter, A., & Eastwood, J. D. (2016). Does state boredom cause failures of attention? Examining the relations between trait boredom, state boredom, and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4749-7.
  110. Hunter, J. A., Abraham, E. H., Hunter, A. G., Goldberg, L. C., & Eastwood, J. D. (2016). Personality and boredom proneness in the prediction of creativity and curiosity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 48–57.Google Scholar
  111. Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 179-236), Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  112. Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Weissinger, E. (1987). Leisure and boredom. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 356–364.Google Scholar
  113. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  114. Jiang, Y., Lianekhammy, J., Lawson, A., Guo, C., Lynam, D., Joseph, J. E., et al. (2009). Brain responses to repeated visual experience among low and high sensation seekers: Role of boredom susceptibility. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 173(2), 100–106.Google Scholar
  115. Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Vocal communication of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 220–235). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  116. Kanevsky, L., & Keighley, T. (2003). To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review: A Journal on Gifted Education, 26, 20–28.Google Scholar
  117. Karmarkar, U. R., & Buonomano, D. V. (2007). Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron, 53, 427–438.Google Scholar
  118. Kass, S. J., Beede, K. E., & Vodanovich, S. J. (2010). Self-report measures of distractibility as correlates of simulated driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42(3), 874–880.Google Scholar
  119. Kecklund, G., & Åkerstedt, T. (1993). Sleepiness in long distance truck driving: An ambulatory EEG study of night driving. Ergonomics, 36(9), 1007–1017.Google Scholar
  120. Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 467–480.Google Scholar
  121. Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006).Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick (eds.), Evolution and Social Psychology (pp. 115–142). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  122. Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932.Google Scholar
  123. Klonowicz, T. (1987). Reactivity and the control of arousal. In J. Strelau & H. J. Eysenck (Eds.), Personality dimensions and arousal (pp. 197–216). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  124. Kroes, S. (2005). Detecting boredom in meetings (pp. 1–5). Enschede: University of Twente.Google Scholar
  125. Lal, S. K., & Craig, A. (2002). Driver fatigue: Electroencephalography and psychological assessment. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 313–321.Google Scholar
  126. Larson, R. W., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Boredom in the middle school years: Blaming schools versus blaming students. American Journal of Education, 99, 418–443.Google Scholar
  127. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  128. Lazzaro, I., Gordon, E., Whitmont, S., Plahn, M., Li, W., Clarke, S., et al. (1998). Quantified EEG activity in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical EEG (electroencephalography), 29(1), 37–42.Google Scholar
  129. Leary, M. R., Rogers, P. A., Canfield, R. W., & Coe, C. (1986). Boredom in interpersonal encounters: Antecedents and social implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 968–975.Google Scholar
  130. Lee, T. W. (1986). Toward the development and validation of a measure of job measurement. Manhattan College Journal of Business, 15, 22–28.Google Scholar
  131. Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotions: A functional view. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 123–126). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  132. Lewinsky, H. (1943). Boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 13, 147–152.Google Scholar
  133. Lhommet, M., & Marsella, S. C. (2015). Expressing emotion through posture and gesture. In R. Calvo, S. D’Mello, J. Gratch, & A. Kappas (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of affective computing (pp. 273–285). Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  134. Lohrmann, K. (2008). Langeweile im Unterricht (Boredom in class). Miinster: Waxmann.Google Scholar
  135. London, H., & Monello, L. (1974). Cognitive manipulation of boredom. In H. London & R. Nisbett (Eds.), Thought and feeling (pp. 44–59). Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
  136. London, H., Schubert, D. S., & Washburn, D. (1972). Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 29–36.Google Scholar
  137. Lundberg, U., Melin, B., Evans, G. W., & Holmberg, L. (1993). Physiological deactivation after two contrasting tasks at a video display terminal: Learning vs. repetitive data entry. Ergonomics, 36, 601–611.Google Scholar
  138. Malkovsky, E., Merrifield, C., Goldberg, Y., & Danckert, J. (2012). Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research, 221(1), 59–67.Google Scholar
  139. Mann, S., & Cadman, R. (2014). Does being bored make us more creative?.Creativity. Research Journal, 26(2), 165–173.Google Scholar
  140. Mann, S., & Robinson, A. (2009). Boredom in the lecture theatre: An investigation into the contributors, moderators and outcomes of boredom amongst university students. British Educational Research Journal, 35(2), 243–258.Google Scholar
  141. Martin, M., Sadlo, G., & Stew, G. (2006). The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(3), 193–211.Google Scholar
  142. Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behaviour. BioEssays, 22, 94–103.Google Scholar
  143. Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence-detection of oscillatory processes. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139–170.Google Scholar
  144. McDaniel, B., D'Mello, S., King, B., Chipman, P., Tapp, K., & Graesser, A. (2007, January). Facial features for affective state detection in learning environments. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 29(29), 467–472.Google Scholar
  145. Meck, W. H. (1983). Selective adjustment of the speed of the clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 171–201.Google Scholar
  146. Mercer, K. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2010). Is boredom associated with problem gambling behaviour? It depends on what you mean by ‘boredom’. International Gambling Studies, 10(1), 91–104.Google Scholar
  147. Merrifield, C., & Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232(2), 481–491.Google Scholar
  148. Mikulas, W. L., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. Psychological Record, 43(1), 3–12.Google Scholar
  149. Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The frame of mind. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  150. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259.Google Scholar
  151. Musharbash, Y. (2007). Boredom, time, and modernity: An example from aboriginal Australia. American Anthropologist, 109(2), 307–317.Google Scholar
  152. Nederkoorn, C., Vancleef, L., Wilkenhöner, A., Claes, L., & Havermans, R. C. (2016). Self-inflicted pain out of boredom. Psychiatry Research, 237, 127–132.Google Scholar
  153. Nesselroade, J. R. (1988). Some implications of the trait–state distinction for the study of development over the life span: The case of personality. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 8, pp. 163–189). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  154. Nett, U. E., Goetz, T., & Daniels, L. M. (2010). What to do when feeling bored?: Students' strategies for coping with boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(6), 626–638.Google Scholar
  155. Neu, J. (1998). Boring from within: Endogenous versus reactive boredom. In W. F. Flack & J. D. Laird (Eds.), Emotions in psychopathology: Theory and research (pp. 158–170). London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  156. Ng, A. H., Liu, Y., Chen, J. Z., & Eastwood, J. D. (2015). Culture and state boredom: A comparison between European Canadians and Chinese. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 13–18.Google Scholar
  157. Noulhiane, M., Mella, N., Samson, S., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2007). How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion, 7(4), 697–704.Google Scholar
  158. Nowlis, V., & Nowlis, H. H. (1956). The description and analysis of mood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 65, 345–355.Google Scholar
  159. O’Hanlon, J. F. (1981). Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica, 49, 161–173.Google Scholar
  160. Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Cambridge: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  161. Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 20–50.Google Scholar
  162. O'Brien, W. (2014). Boredom. Analysis, 74, 236–244. doi: 10.1093/analys/anu041.Google Scholar
  163. Ornstein, R. E. (1970). On the experience of time. Baltimore: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  164. Oswald, I. (1962). The EEG of sleep. In I. Oswald (Ed.), Sleeping and waking: Physiology and psychology (pp. 35–41). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  165. Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P., Briner, R. B., & Reynolds, S. (1996). Changing moods: The psychology of mood and mood regulation. London: Longman.Google Scholar
  166. Passik, S. D., Inman, A., Kirsch, K., Theobald, D., & Dickerson, P. (2003). Initial validation of a scale to measure purposelessness, understimulation, and boredom in cancer patients: Toward a redefinition of depression in advanced disease. Palliative and Supportive Care, 1, 41–50.Google Scholar
  167. Pattyn, N., Neyt, X., Henderickx, D., & Soetens, E. (2008). Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiology & Behavior, 93(1), 369–378.Google Scholar
  168. Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-nega- tive asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction be- tween affective and informational negativity ef- fects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Eu- ropean review of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33–60). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  169. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L. M., Stupnisky, R. H., & Perry, R. P. (2010). Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(3), 531–549.Google Scholar
  170. Perkins, R. E., & Hill, A. B. (1985). Cognitive and affective aspects of boredom. The British Psychological Society, 76, 221–234.Google Scholar
  171. Phipps-Nelson, J., Redman, J. R., & Rajaratnam, S. M. (2011). Temporal profile of prolonged, night-time driving performance: Breaks from driving temporarily reduce time-on-task fatigue but not sleepiness. Journal of Sleep Research, 20(3), 404–415.Google Scholar
  172. Pouthas, V., & Perbal, S. (2004). Time perception does not only depend on accurate clock mechanisms but also on unimpaired attention and memory processes. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 367–385.Google Scholar
  173. Poynter, W. D. (1983). Duration judgment and the segmentation of experience. Memory & Cognition, 11, 77–82.Google Scholar
  174. Raccanello, D., & Bianchetti, C. (2014). Pictorial representations of achievement emotions: Preliminary data with children and adults. In T. Mascio, R. Gennari, P. Vitorini, R. Vicari, & F. de la Prieta (Eds.), Methodologies and Intelligent Systems for Technology Enhanced Learning (pp. 127–134). Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
  175. Ragheb, M. G., & Merydith, S. P. (2001). Development and validation of a unidimensional scale measuring free time boredom. Leisure Studies, 20, 41–59.Google Scholar
  176. Raposa, M. L. (1999). Boredom and the religious imagination. Charlottesville: University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
  177. Reisenzein, R. (2000). Exploring the strength of association between the components of emotion syndromes: The case of surprise. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 1–38.Google Scholar
  178. Robinson, W. P. (1975). Boredom at school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 45(2), 141–152.Google Scholar
  179. Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology, Emotions, relationships, and health (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36). Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
  180. Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of General Psychology, 2, 247–270.Google Scholar
  181. Rosenberg, E., & Ekman, P. (1994). Coherence between expressive and experiential systems in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 201–229.Google Scholar
  182. Roy, D. (1959). " Banana Time": Job Satisfaction and Informal Interaction. Human Organization, 18(4), 158–168.Google Scholar
  183. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296–320.Google Scholar
  184. Rubia, K. (2011). “cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e69–e87.Google Scholar
  185. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.Google Scholar
  186. Russell, B. (1996). The conquest of happiness. New York: W. W Norton & Company Inc..Google Scholar
  187. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.Google Scholar
  188. Sangal, R. B., & Sangal, J. M. (2015). Use of EEG Beta-1 power and theta/Beta ratio over Broca’s area to confirm diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 46(3), 177–182.Google Scholar
  189. Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 370–390.Google Scholar
  190. Scerbo, M. W., Greenwald, C. Q., & Sawin, D. A. (1992). Vigilance: It’s boring, it’s difficult and I can’t do anything about it. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 36th Annual Meeting, 36, 1508–1512.Google Scholar
  191. Scherer, K. R. (1993). Studying the emotion-antecedent appraisal process: An expert system approach. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 325–355.Google Scholar
  192. Scherer, K. R. (2013). Nonlinguistic vocal indicators of emotion and psychopathology. In C. Izard (Ed.), Emotions in personality and psychopathology (pp. 495–529). New York and London: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  193. Scherer, K. R., & Ellgring, H. (2007). Multimodal expression of emotion: Affect programs or componential appraisal patterns? Emotion, 7(1), 158–171.Google Scholar
  194. Schier, M. A. (2000). Changes in EEG alpha power during simulated driving: A demonstration. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 155–162.Google Scholar
  195. Schueller, S. M. (2014). Person-activity fit in positive psychological interventions. In A. C. Parks & S. M. Schueller (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of positive psychological interventions (pp. 385-399). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.Google Scholar
  196. Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  197. Schwarz, N. (2000). Emotion, cognition, and decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 433–440.Google Scholar
  198. Schweizer, T. S. (2006). The psychology of novelty-seeking, creativity and innovation: Neurocognitive aspects within a work-psychological perspective. Creativity and Innovation Management, 15(2), 164–172.Google Scholar
  199. Seel, R. T., & Kreutzer, J. S. (2003). Depression assessment after traumatic brain injury: An empirically based classification method. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(11), 1621–1628.Google Scholar
  200. Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 57–60.Google Scholar
  201. Skowronski, M. (2012). When the bored behave badly (or exceptionally). Personnel Review, 41, 143–159.Google Scholar
  202. Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanation. Psychological Re- view, 105, 131–142.Google Scholar
  203. Smith, R. P. (1981). Boredom: A review. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 23, 329–340.Google Scholar
  204. Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233–269.Google Scholar
  205. Smith, J. L., Wagaman, J., & Handley, I. M. (2009). Keeping it dull or making it fun: Task variation as a function of promotion versus prevention focus. Motivation and Emotion, 33(2), 150–160.Google Scholar
  206. Spielberger, C. D. (1966a). The effects of anxiety on complex learning and academic achievement. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 361–398). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  207. Spielberger, C. D. (1966b). Theory and research on anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 3–20). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  208. Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 1, pp. 23–49). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  209. Steinberger, F., Moeller, A., & Schroeter, R. (2016). The antecedents, experience, and coping strategies of driver boredom in young adult males. Journal of Safety Research, 59, 69–82.Google Scholar
  210. Struk, A. A., Scholer, A. A., & Danckert, J. (2016). A self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion, 30(8), 1388–1401.Google Scholar
  211. Sundberg, N. D., Latkin, C. A., Farmer, R. F., & Saoud, J. (1991). Boredom in young adults gender and cultural comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22(2), 209–223.Google Scholar
  212. Taatgen, N. A., van Rijn, H., & Anderson, J. R. (2007). An integrated theory of prospective time interval estimation: The role of cognition, attention and learning. Psychological Review, 114, 577–598.Google Scholar
  213. Tabatabaie, A. F., Azadehfar, M. R., Mirian, N., Noroozian, M., Yoonessi, A., & Yoonessi, A. (2014). Neural correlates of boredom in music perception. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 5(4), 259–266.Google Scholar
  214. Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P., & Touchstone, R. M. (1977). Physiological, subjective and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. FAA Office of Aviation Medicine Reports, 9, 75–78.Google Scholar
  215. Todman, M. (2003). Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry, 66, 146–167.Google Scholar
  216. Todman, M. (2013). The dimensions of state boredom: Frequency, duration, unpleasantness, consequences and causal attributions. Educational Research International, 1(1), 32–40.Google Scholar
  217. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 103–129). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  218. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs, 77, 1–31.Google Scholar
  219. Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.Google Scholar
  220. Troutwine, R., & O’Neal, E. C. (1981). Volition, performance of a boring task and time estimation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52(3), 865–866.Google Scholar
  221. Tze, V. M. C., Daniels, L. M., Klassen, R. M., & Johnson, C.-H. L. (2013). Canadian and Chinese university students’ approaches to coping with academic boredom. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 32–43.Google Scholar
  222. van Aart, J., Bartneck, C., Hu, J., Rauterberg, M., & Salem, B. (2010). How to behave as Alice in wonderland–about boredom and curiosity. Entertainment Computing, 1, 125–137. doi: 10.1016/j.entcom.2010.09.003.Google Scholar
  223. van Hooff, M. L., & van Hooft, E. A. (2014). Boredom at work: Proximal and distal consequences of affective work-related boredom. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 19(3), 348.Google Scholar
  224. Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2011). On boredom and social identity: A pragmatic meaning-regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1679–1691.Google Scholar
  225. Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2012). On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36(2), 181–194.Google Scholar
  226. Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2016). Going to political extremes in response to boredom. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(6), 687–699.Google Scholar
  227. Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2017a). Boredom begs to differ: Differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion, 17(2), 309–322.Google Scholar
  228. Van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2017b). Can boredom help? Increased prosocial intentions in response to boredom. Self and Identity, 16(1), 82–96.Google Scholar
  229. Van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., & Sedikides, C. (2013). In search of meaningfulness: Nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion, 13(3), 450–461.Google Scholar
  230. Vodanovich, S. J. (2003). Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology, 137(6), 569–595.Google Scholar
  231. Vodanovich, S. J., & Kass, S. J. (1990). A factor analytic study of the boredom proneness scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 115–123.Google Scholar
  232. Vodanovich, S. J., & Watt, J. D. (2015). Self-report measures of boredom: An updated review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology, 150(2), 196–228. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2015.1074531.Google Scholar
  233. Vodanovich, S. J., Watt, J. D., & Piotrowski, C. (1997). Boredom proneness in African-American college students: A factor analytic perspective. Education, 118(2), 229–236.Google Scholar
  234. Vogel-Walcutt, J. J., Fiorella, L., Carper, T., & Schatz, S. (2012). The definition, assessment, and mitigation of state boredom within educational settings: A comprehensive review. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 89–111.Google Scholar
  235. Wackermann, J., & Ehm, W. (2006). The dual klepsydra model of internal time representation and time reproduction. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239, 482–493.Google Scholar
  236. Walker, E. L. (1980). Psychological complexity and preference. Monterey: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  237. Wallace, J. C., Vodanovich, S. J., & Restino, B. M. (2003). Predicting cognitive failures from boredom proneness and daytime sleepiness scores: An investigation within military and undergraduate samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(4), 635–644.Google Scholar
  238. Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Psychology, 28, 879–896.Google Scholar
  239. Wangh, M. (1975). Boredom in psychoanalytic perspective. Sociological Research, 42, 538–550.Google Scholar
  240. Watt, J. D. (1991). Effect of boredom proneness on time perception. Psychological Reports, 69(1), 323–327.Google Scholar
  241. Wearden, J., & Penton-Voak, I. (1995). Feeling the heat: Body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. B, 48, 129–141.Google Scholar
  242. Weinstein, L., Xie, X., & Cleanthous, C. C. (1995). Purpose in life, boredom, and volunteerism in a group of retirees. Psychological Reports, 76(2), 482–482.Google Scholar
  243. Weissinger, E., Caldwell, L. L., & Bandalos, D. L. (1992). Relation between intrinsic motivation and boredom in leisure time. Leisure Sciences, 14, 317–325.Google Scholar
  244. Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346.Google Scholar
  245. Wilson, T. D., Reinhard, D. A., Westgate, E. C., Gilbert, D. T., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., Brown, C. L., & Shaked, A. (2014). Social psychology. Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind. Science, 345, 75–77.Google Scholar
  246. Wittmann, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1955–1967.Google Scholar
  247. Wittmann, M., & van Wassenhove, V. (2009). The experience of time: Neural mechanisms and the interplay of emotion, cognition and embodiment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1809–1813.Google Scholar
  248. Yoshida, K., Sawamura, D., Inagaki, Y., Ogawa, K., Ikoma, K., & Sakai, S. (2014). Brain activity during the flow experience: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroscience Letters, 573, 30–34.Google Scholar
  249. Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (2004). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: An executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64(3), 319–328.Google Scholar
  250. Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(1), 12–16.Google Scholar
  251. Zhao, C., Zhao, M., Liu, J., & Zheng, C. (2012). Electroencephalogram and electrocardiograph assessment of mental fatigue in a driving simulator. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 45, 83–90.Google Scholar
  252. Zuckerman, M. (1976). General and situation-specific traits and states: New approaches to assessment of anxiety and other constructs. In M. Zuckerman & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Emotion and anxiety: New concepts, methods, and applications (pp. 133–174). Hillsdale: Errbaum.Google Scholar
  253. Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  254. Zuckerman, M. (1983). The distinction between trait and state scales is not arbitrary: Comment on Allen and Potkay's "on the arbitrary distinction between traits and states.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1083–1086.Google Scholar
  255. Zuckerman, M. (1987). A critical look at three arousal constructs in personality theories: Optimal levels of arousal, strength of the nervous system, and sensitivities to signals of reward and punishment. In J. Strelau & H. J. Eysenck (Eds.), Personality dimensions and arousal (pp. 217–232). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of LouisvilleLouisvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations