Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences

, Volume 16, Issue 5, pp 785–809 | Cite as

Enacting musical emotions. sense-making, dynamic systems, and the embodied mind

  • Andrea Schiavio
  • Dylan van der Schyff
  • Julian Cespedes-Guevara
  • Mark Reybrouck
Article

Abstract

The subject of musical emotions has emerged only recently as a major area of research. While much work in this area offers fascinating insights to musicological research, assumptions about the nature of emotional experience seem to remain committed to appraisal, representations, and a rule-based or information-processing model of cognition. Over the past three decades alternative ‘embodied’ and ‘enactive’ models of mind have challenged this approach by emphasising the self-organising aspects of cognition, often describing it as an ongoing process of dynamic interactivity between an organism and its environment. More recently, this perspective has been applied to the study of emotion in general, opening up interesting new possibilities for theory and research. This new approach, however, has received rather limited attention in musical contexts. With this in mind, we critically review the history of music and emotion studies, arguing that many existing theories offer only limited views of what musical-emotional experience entails. We then attempt to provide preliminary grounding for an alternative perspective on music and emotion based on the enactive/dynamic systems approach to the study of mind.

Keywords

Music and emotion Embodiment Sense-making Dynamic systems Enaction Embodied music cognition 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Fred Cummins for providing us with insights that helped greatly in the early stages of preparing this manuscript. We are also grateful to Renee Timmers, Tom Cochrane, Giovanna Colombetti, and to the anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and suggestions.

References

  1. Baker, F. A., Gleadhill, L., & Dingle, G. A. (2007). Music therapy and emotional exploration: exposing substance abuse clients to the experiences of non-drug-induced emotions. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 34(4), 321–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology Inc., 10(1), 20–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barrett, L. (2011). Beyond the brain: How body and environment shape animal and human minds. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative architecture of the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 361–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Smith, E. R. (2010). The context principle. In The mind in context (pp. 1–22). New York, NY: Guildford.Google Scholar
  6. Beer, R. D. (1995). A dynamical systems perspective on agent-environment interaction. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 173–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bohlman, P. (1999). Ontologies of music. In N. Cook & M. Everist (Eds.), Rethinking music (pp. 17–34). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bowman, W. (2004). Cognition and the body: Perspectives from music education. In L. Bresler (Ed.), Knowing bodies, moving minds: Toward embodied teaching and learning (pp. 29–50). Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brattico, E., Bogert, B. & Jacobsen, T. (2013). Toward a neural chronometry for the aesthetic experi- ence of music. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(206). doi:  10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00206.
  10. Budd, M. (1989). Music and the communication of emotion. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 47(2), 129–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  12. Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  13. Clark, A. (2001). Mindware: An introduction to the philosophy of cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Clarke, E., & Clarke, D. (Eds.). (2011). Music and consciousness. Philosophical, psychological and cultural perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Clarke, E., Dibben, N., & Pitts, S. (2010). Music and mind in everyday life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Clarkson, M. G., & Clifton, R. K. (1985). Infant pitch perception: evidence for responding to pitch categories and the missing fundamental. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 1521–1528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in Emotion; always, sometimes or never? In L. Nadel, R. Lane, & G. L. Ahern (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 24–61). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Cochrane, T. (2010a). A simulation theory of musical expressivity. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 88(2), 191–207. doi: 10.1080/00048400902941257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cochrane, T. (2010b). Using the persona to express complex emotions in music. Music Analysis, 29(1–3), 264–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Cochrane, T., Fantini, B., & Scherer, K. R. (2013). The emotional power of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Colombetti, G. (2014). The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Colombetti, G., & Thompson, E. (2008). The feeling body: Toward an enactive approach to emotion. In W. F. Overton, U. Muller, & J. L. Newman (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on embodiment and consciousness (pp. 45–68). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Ass.Google Scholar
  23. Cone, E. T. (1974). The composer’s voice. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  24. Cook, N. (2001). Theorizing musical meaning. Music Theory Spectrum, 23(2), 170–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Costa-Giomi, E. (2013). Perceiving differences and similarities in music: Melodic categorization during the first years of life. Perspectives: Journal of the Early Childhood Music & Movement Association, 8(2).Google Scholar
  26. Cross, I. (1999). Is music the most important thing we ever did? Music, development and evolution. In S. W. Yi (Ed.), Music, mind and science (pp. 10–39). Seoul: Seoul National University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Cross, I. (2001). Music, mind and evolution. Psychology of Music, 29, 95–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. D’Ausilio, A. (2007). The role of the mirror system in mapping complex sounds into actions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 5847–5848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. D’Ausilio, A. (2009). Mirror-like mechanisms and music. The Scientific World Journal, 9, 1415–1422. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2009.160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
  31. Davies, S. (1994). Musical meaning and expression. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Davies, S. (1997). Contra the hypothetical persona in music. In M. Hjort & S. Laver (Eds.), Emotion and the arts (pp. 95–109). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Davies, S. (2010). Emotions expressed and aroused by music: Philosophical perspectives. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 15–43). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. A. (2007). Participatory sense-making: an enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Dennett, D. (1978). Brainstorms. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  36. Di Paolo, E. (2005). Autopoiesis, adaptivity, teleology, agency. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 429–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Di Paolo, E. (2009). Extended life. Topoi, 28, 9–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Dibben, N. (2004). The role of peripheral feedback in emotional experience with music. Music Perception, 22(1), 79–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Dumas, G., Laroche, J., & Lehmann, A. (2014). Your body, my body, our coupling moves our bodies. Froniers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1004. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01004.Google Scholar
  40. Eerola, T., & Vuoskoski, J. K. (2013). A review of music and emotion studies: approaches, emotion models, and stimuli. Music Perception, 30(3), 307–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3(4), 364–370. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410740.
  42. Elliott, D., & Silverman, M. (2014). Music matters: A philosophy of music education (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Fabian, D., Timmers, R., & Schubert, E. (2014). Expressiveness in music performance: Empirical approaches across styles and cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Fantasia, V., De Jaegher, H., & Fasulo, A. (2014). We can work it out: an enactive look at cooperation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00874.
  45. Fogel, A., Nwokah, E., Dedo, J. Y., Messinger, D., Dickson, K. L., Mayusov, E., & Holt, S. A. (1992). Social process theory of emotion: a dynamic systems approach. Social Development, 1, 122–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Freeman, W. (1999). How brains make up their minds. London: Orion Press.Google Scholar
  47. Freeman, W. (2000). A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  48. Friston, K. (2009). The free energy principle: a rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(1), 293–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Gentsch, A., & Synofzik, M. (2014). Affective coding: the emotional dimension of agency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 608. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00608.
  50. Gerson*, S., Schiavio*, A., Timmers, R. & Hunnius, S. (2015). Active drumming experience increases infants’ sensitivity to audiovisual synchronicity during observed drumming actions (*both first author). PLoS ONE, 10(6): e0130960.Google Scholar
  51. Gil, V., Reybrouck, M., Tejada, J., & Verschaffel, L. (2015). Improving the representational strategies of children in a music listening and playing task: an intervention-based study. Research Studies in Music Education, 37(1), 77–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Granic, I. (2000). The self-organization of parent–child relations: Beyond bi-directional relations. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development and self-organization (pp. 267–297). Cambridge: Cambridge UP.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Haken, H. (1977). Synergetics - an introduction: Non equilibrium phase transitions and self-organization in physics. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Hurley, S. (1998). Consciousness in action. London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930(1), 43–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  57. Husserl, E. (1989). Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy. [Second Book: Studies in the Phenomenology of Constitution]. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  58. Hutto, D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  59. Iacoboni, M. (2008). Mirroring people: The new science of how we connect with others. New York: Farrar, Strauss & Giroux.Google Scholar
  60. Innes-Ker, A., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2002). Emotion concepts and emotional states in social judgment and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 804–816. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.4.804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Johnson, M. (2007). The meaning of the body: Aesthetics of human understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Juslin, P. N. (2013a). From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: towards a unified theory of musical emotions. Physics of Life Reviews, 10, 235–266. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2013.05.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Juslin, P. N. (2013b). What does music express? Basic emotions and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 770–814. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.). (2001). Music and emotion: Theory, and research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J. A. (2010). Introduction: Aims, organization, and terminology. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion (pp. 3–14). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  68. Juslin, P. N., & Timmers, R. (2010). Expression and communication of emotion in music performance. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 453–489). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  69. Juslin, P. N., & Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 559–575. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X08006079. discussion 575–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Juslin, P. N., Liljeström, S., & Västfjäll, D. (2010). How does music evoke emotions? Exploring the underlying mechanisms. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 605–642). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Kelso, S. (1995). Dynamic patterns. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  72. Khalfa, S., & Peretz, I. (2004). Electrodermal responses to dissonant and consonant music. Polksie Forum Psychologiczne, 9(1), 63–72.Google Scholar
  73. Kiverstein, J, & Miller, M. (2015). The embodied brain: towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9(237). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00237.
  74. Kivy, P. (1989). Sound sentiment. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Kivy, P. (1999). Feeling the musical emotions. British Journal of Aesthetics, 39, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Koelsch, (2013). Striking a chord in the brain: the neurophysiological correlates of music-evoked positive emotions. In T. Cochrane, B. Fantini, and K. Scherer (Eds.). The Emotional Power of Music: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Musical Expression, Arousal, and Social Control (pp. 177-196). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Krueger, J. (2009). Enacting musical experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(2–3), 98–123.Google Scholar
  78. Krueger, J. (2011). Doing things with music. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Krueger, J. (2013). Empathy, enaction, and shared musical experience: Evidence from infant cognition. In T. Cochrane, B. Fantini, & K. Scherer (Eds.), The emotional power of music: Multidisciplinary perspectives on musical arousal, expression, and social control (pp. 177–196). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Krueger, J. (2014a). Affordances and the musically extended mind. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(1003), 1–12.Google Scholar
  81. Krueger, J. (2014b). Emotions and the social niche. In C. von Scheve & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 156–171). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Krueger, J. (2014c). Varieties of extended emotions. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 533–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Krueger, J. (2015a). Musicing, materiality, and the emotional niche. Action, Criticism, and Theory for Music Education., 14(3), 43–62.Google Scholar
  84. Krueger, J. (2015b). Empathy beyond the head: comment on “music, empathy, and cultural understanding” by E. Clarke et al. Physics of Life Reviews, 15, 92–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 51(4), 336–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Labbé, C., & Grandjean, D. (2014). Musical emotions predicted by feelings of entrainment. Music Perception, 32(2), 170–185. doi: 10.1525/MP.2014.32.2.170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Laible, D., & Thompson, R. (2000). Mother–child discourse, attachment security, shared positive affect, and early conscience development. Child Development, 71, 1424–1440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Lang, P. J. (1994). The varieties of emotional experience: a meditation on James–Lange theory. Psychological Review, 101(2), 211–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Lange, C. (1887). Ueber Gemüthsbewegungen. Eine psycho-physiologische Studie. First appeared in Rand, Benjamin (Ed.)(1912). The Classical Psychologists (pp. 672–684). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  90. Laske, O.-E. (1977). Music, memory and thought. Explorations in Cognitive Musicology University of Pittsburgh. Ann Arbor: University Microfilms International.Google Scholar
  91. Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37(9), 1019–1024. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.37.9.1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Leech-Wilkinson, D. (2013). The emotional power of music performance. In T. Cochrane, B. Fantini, & K. Scherer (Eds.), The emotional power of music: Multidisciplinary perspectives on musical arousal, expression, and social control (pp. 41–54). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Leman, M. (2007). Embodied music cognition and mediation technology. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  94. Leman, M. & Maes, P.-J. (2015). The role of embodiment in the perception of music. Empirical Musicology Review, 99(3–4), 236–246.Google Scholar
  95. Levinson, J. (1996). The pleasure of aesthetics. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
  96. Lewis, M. D. (2000). Emotional organization at three timescales. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development and self-organization (pp. 37–69). Cambridge: Cambridge UP.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Lundqvist, L.-O., Carlsson, F., Hilmersson, P., & Juslin, P. N. (2008). Emotional responses to music: experience, expression, and physiology. Psychology of Music, 37(1), 61–90. doi: 10.1177/0305735607086048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Maiese, M. (2011). Embodiment, emotion, and cognition. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Martin, P. J. (1995). Sounds and society: Themes in the sociology of music. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
  100. Matravers, D. (1998). Art and emotion. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  101. Matyja, J., & Schiavio, A. (2013). Enactive music cognition: background and research themes. Constructivist. Foundations, 8, 351–357.Google Scholar
  102. Menin, D., & Schiavio, A. (2012). Rethinking musical affordances. AVANT. Trends in interdisciplinary studies, 3(2), 202–215.Google Scholar
  103. Merleau-Ponty, M. (2002). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  104. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  105. Moors, A. (2013). On the causal role of appraisal in emotion. Emotion Review, 5(2), 132–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002–1005. doi: 10.1126/science.1136930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Novembre, G., Ticini, L. F., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Keller, P. E. (2014). Motor simulation and the coordination of joint actions in real time. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1062–1068. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Nussbaum, C. O. (2007). The musical representation: Meaning, ontology, and emotion. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  109. O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001a). A sensorimotor approach to vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001b). What it is like to see: a sensorimotor theory of visual experience. Synthese, 129(1), 79–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Overy, K. & Molnar-Szakacs, I. (2009). Being together in time: musical experience and the mirror neuron system. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26(5), 489–504. doi: 10.1525/mp.2009.26.5.489.
  112. Panksepp, J., & Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: the neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behavioural Processes, 60, 133–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Patel, A. (2008). Music, language and the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  114. Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: movement influences infants’ rhythm perception. Science, 308, 5727. 1430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Plamper, J. (2015). The history of emotions. An introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  116. Ramachandran, V. S. (2011). The tell tale brain: A neuroscientist’s quest for what makes us human. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  117. Reybrouck, M. (2001). Biological roots of musical epistemology: functional Cycles, Umwelt, and enactive listening. Semiotica, 134(1–4), 599–633.Google Scholar
  118. Reybrouck, M. (2005). A biosemiotic and ecological approach to music cognition: event perception between auditory listening and cognitive economy. Axiomathes. An International Journal in Ontology and Cognitive Systems, 15(2), 229–266.Google Scholar
  119. Reybrouck, M. (2006). Music cognition and the bodily approach: musical instruments as tools for musical semantics. Contemporary Music Review, 25(1/2), 59–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Reybrouck, M. (2010). Music cognition and real-time listening: denotation, cue abstraction, route description and cognitive maps. Musicae Scientiae, 14, 187–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Reybrouck, M. (2014). Music teaching and learning: Reflecting on the musical experience. In T. De Baets & T. Buchborn (Eds.), The reflective music teacher (pp. 57–70). Innsbruck: Helbling.Google Scholar
  122. Reybrouck, M. (2015). Music as environment: an ecological and biosemiotic approach. Behavioral Sciences, 5, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Reybrouck, M., & Brattico, E. (2015). Neuroplasticity beyond sounds: neural adaptations following long-term musical aesthetic experiences. Brain Sciences, 5, 69–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Russell, J. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145–172. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks : The Official Journal of the International Neural Network Society, 18(4), 317–352. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2005.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Scherer, K. R. (2004). Which emotions can be induced by music? What are the underlying mechanisms? And how can we measure them? Journal of New Music Research, 33(3), 239–251. doi: 10.1080/0929821042000317822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 44(4), 695–729. doi: 10.1177/0539018405058216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Scherer, K. R. (2008). Music evoked emotions are different – more often aesthetic than utilitarian. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Scherer, K. R., & Coutinho, E. (2013). How music creates emotion: A multifactorial process approach. In T. Cochrane, B. Fantini, & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), The emotional power of music, multidisciplinary perspectives on musical arousal, expression, and social control (pp. 121–145). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Scherer, K. R., & Zentner, M. R. (2001). Emotional effects of music: Production rules. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 361–392). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  132. Schiavio, A. (2012). Constituting the musical object. A neurophenomenological perspective on musical research. Teorema, 13(3), 63–80.Google Scholar
  133. Schiavio, A. (2014). Action, enaction, inter(en)action. Empirical Musicology Review, 9(3–4), 254–262.Google Scholar
  134. Schiavio*, A. & Høffding*, S. (2015). Playing together without communicating? A pre-reflective and enactive account of joint musical performance (*both first author). Musicae Scientiae, 19(4), 366–388.Google Scholar
  135. Schiavio, A., & Altenmüller, E. (2015). Exploring music-based rehabilitation for Parkinsonism through embodied cognitive science. Frontiers in Neurology, 6(217):  10.3389/fneur.2015.00217.
  136. Schiavio, A., & Cummins, F. (2015). An inter(en)active approach to musical agency and learning, in Timmers, R., Dibben, N., Eitan, Z., Granot, R., Metcalfe, T., Schiavio, A., & Williamson, V. (Eds). Proceedings of the International Conference on the Multimodal Experience of Music 2015. Google Scholar
  137. Schiavio, A., & Gerson, S. (2015). Una prospettiva enattiva sulla musicalità infantile. In G. Ruggiero & D. Bruni (Eds.), Il Ritmo della Mente. La Musica tra Scienza Cognitiva e Psicoterapia (pp. 119–139). Mimesis Edizioni: Milan.Google Scholar
  138. Schiavio, A., Menin, D., & Matyja, J. (2015). Music in the flesh: embodied simulation in musical understanding. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 24(4), 340–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Schubert, E. (2007). The influence of emotion, locus of emotion and familiarity upon preference in music. Psychology of Music, 35(3), 499–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Schubert, E. (2013). Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  141. Seth, A. K., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Extending predictive processing to the body: emotion as interoceptive inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(03), 227–228.Google Scholar
  142. Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2010). Thinking in movement. Further analyses and validations. In J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, & E. A. Di Paolo (Eds.), Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science (pp. 165–182). Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2012). Fundamental and inherently interrelated aspects of animation. In A. Foolen, U. Lüdtke, T. Racine, & J. Zlatev (Eds.), Moving ourselves, moving others: Motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and language (pp. 27–56). Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Sinigaglia, C., & Sparaci, L. (2010). Emotions in action through the looking glass. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 55, 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Sloboda, J. A. (2000). Musical performance and emotion: Issues and developments. In S. W. Yi (Ed.), Music, mind and science (pp. 220–238). Seoul: Western Music Research Institute.Google Scholar
  146. Sloboda, J. A., & Juslin, P. N. (2001). Psychological perspectives on music and emotion. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 71–104). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  147. Solomon, R. C. (1976). The passions: Emotions and the meaning of life. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  148. Stewart, J., Gapenne, O., & Di Paolo, E. A. (Eds.). (2010). Enaction: Toward a new paradigm for cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  149. Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. Strogatz, S. (1994). Nonlinear dynamics and chaos: With applications to physics, biology, chemistry, and engineering. Perseus Books.Google Scholar
  151. Strogatz, S. (2001). Exploring complex networks. Nature, 410(6825), 268–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  153. Thompson, E. (2005). Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 407–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  154. Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  155. Thompson, W. F., & Quinto, L. (2011). Music and emotion: Psychological considerations. In P. Schellekens & E. Goldie (Eds.), The aesthetic mind: Philosophy and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  156. Thompson, E., & Stapleton, M. (2009). Making sense of sense-making: reflections on enactive and extended mind theories. Topoi, 28(1), 23–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Trainor, L. J., & Trehub, S. E. (1994). Key membership and implied harmony in Western tonal music: developmental perspectives. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 125–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Trehub, S. E., & Thorpe, L. A. (1989). Infants’ perception of rhythm: categorization of auditory sequences by temporal structure. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 217–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Trevarthen, C. (2002). Origins of musical identity: Evidence from infancy for musical social awareness. In R. A. R. MacDonald, D. J. Hargreaves, & D. Miell (Eds.), Musical Identities (pp. 21–38) Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  160. van der Schyff, D. (2013a). Music, culture and the evolution of the human mind: Looking beyond dichotomies. Hellenic Journal of Music, Education and Culture, 4(1).Google Scholar
  161. van der Schyff, D. (2013b). Emotion, embodied mind, and the therapeutic aspects of musical experience in everyday life. Approaches: Music Therapy & Special Music Education, 5(1), 20–58.Google Scholar
  162. van der Schyff, D. (2013c). Music, meaning and the embodied mind: Towards and enactive approach to music cognition. Unpublished MA thesis, The University of Sheffield, UK.Google Scholar
  163. van der Schyff, D. (2015). Music as a manifestation of life: exploring enactivism and the ‘eastern perspective’ for music education. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 345. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00345.Google Scholar
  164. van der Schyff, D., Schiavio, A. & Elliott, D.J. (in press). Critical ontology for an enactive music pedagogy. Action, Criticism & Theory for Music Education. Google Scholar
  165. van Gelder, T. J., & Port, R. F. (1995). It’s about time: And overview of the dynamical approach to cognition. In R. F. Port & T. J. van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as motion (pp. 1–43). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  166. Varela, F. (1979). Principles of biological autonomy. Boston: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  167. Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  168. Von Uexkull, J. (1934). A foray into the worlds of animals and humans. With a theory of meaning. Minneapolis, MN: University on Minnesota Press [2010].Google Scholar
  169. Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion, 8(4), 494–521. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Andrea Schiavio
    • 1
    • 2
  • Dylan van der Schyff
    • 3
  • Julian Cespedes-Guevara
    • 2
  • Mark Reybrouck
    • 4
  1. 1.School of MusicThe Ohio State UniversityColumbusUSA
  2. 2.Department of MusicThe University of SheffieldSheffieldUK
  3. 3.Faculty of EducationSimon Fraser UniversityBurnabyCanada
  4. 4.Section of MusicologyKU Leuven - University of LeuvenLeuvenBelgium

Personalised recommendations