Advertisement

Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences

, Volume 10, Issue 1, pp 1–22 | Cite as

Doing things with music

  • Joel W. Krueger
Article

Abstract

This paper is an exploration of how we do things with music—that is, the way that we use music as an “esthetic technology” to enact micro-practices of emotion regulation, communicative expression, identity construction, and interpersonal coordination that drive core aspects of our emotional and social existence. The main thesis is: from birth, music is directly perceived as an affordance-laden structure. Music, I argue, affords a sonic world, an exploratory space or “nested acoustic environment” that further affords possibilities for, among other things, (1) emotion regulation and (2) social coordination. When we do things with music, we are engaged in the work of creating and cultivating the self, as well as creating and cultivating a shared world that we inhabit with others. I develop this thesis by first introducing the notion of a “musical affordance”. Next, I look at how “emotional affordances” in music are exploited to construct and regulate emotions. I summon empirical research on neonate music therapy to argue that this is something we emerge from the womb knowing how to do. I then look at “social affordances” in music, arguing that joint attention to social affordances in music alters how music is both perceived and appropriated by joint attenders within social listening contexts. In support, I describe the experience of listening to and engaging with music in a live concert setting. Thinking of music as an affordance-laden structure thus reaffirms the crucial role that music plays in constructing and regulating emotional and social experiences in everyday life.

Keywords

Music Emotion Enaction Affordance Intersubjectivity Joint attention 

References

  1. Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In H. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in other-infant interaction (pp. 271–289). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  2. Bunt, L., & Pavlicevic, M. (2001). Music and emotion: Perspectives from music therapy. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Campbell, J. (2002). Reference and consciousness. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Clayton, M., Sager, R., & Will, U. (2004). In time with the music: The concept of entrainment and its significance for ethnomusicology. ESEM Counterpoint, 1, 1–82.Google Scholar
  5. Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 15(2), 181–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Clarke, E. F. (2005). Ways of listening: An ecological approach to the perception of musical meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Cochrane, T. (2009). Joint attention to music. British Journal of Aesthetics, 49(1), 59–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Collins, S., & Kuck, K. (1991). Music therapy in the neonatal intensive care unit. Neonatal Network, 9(6), 23–26.Google Scholar
  9. Costall, A. (1986). The “psychologist’s fallacy” in ecological realism. Teorie & Modelli, 3, 37–46.Google Scholar
  10. Costall, A. (1995). Socializing affordances. Theory Psychology, 5(4), 467–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cross, I. (2006). Music and social being. Musicology Australia, XXVIII, 114–126.Google Scholar
  12. Davies, S. (2001). Philosophical perspectives on music’s expressiveness. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Theory and research (pp. 23–44). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  13. DeCasper, A., & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. DeCasper, A., & Spence, M. (1986). Newborns prefer a familiar story over an unfamiliar one. Infant Behavior & Development, 9, 133–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. DeNora, T. (2000). Music in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fritz, T., Jentschke, S., Gosselin, N., Sammler, D., Peretz, I., Turner, R., et al. (2009). Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Current Biology, 19(7), 573–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  18. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  19. Hamilton, A. (2007). Aesthetics and music. New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
  20. Haslebeck, F. (2004). Music therapy with preterm infants—Theoretical approach and first practical experience. Music Therapy Today (online), 5(1), 1–15. Retrieved August 3, 2009, from http://www.musictherapyworld.de.Google Scholar
  21. Heft, H. (2001). Ecological psychology in context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the legacy of William James’s radical empiricism. Manwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Hicks, F. (1992). The power of music. Nursing Times, 88, 72–74.Google Scholar
  23. Kaminski, J., & Hall, W. (1996). The effect of soothing music on neonatal behavioral states in the hospital newborn nursery. Neonatal Network, 16, 45–54.Google Scholar
  24. Katz, S. (1987). Is Gibson a relativist? In A. Costall & A. Still (Eds.), Cognitive in question (pp. 115–127). Brighton: Harvester.Google Scholar
  25. Krueger, J. (2009). Enacting musical experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(2–3), 98–123.Google Scholar
  26. Levinson, J. (2005). Musical expressiveness as hearability-as-expression. In M. Kieran (Ed.), Contemporary debates in aesthetics and the philosophy of art (pp. 192–206). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  27. Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.). (2009). Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Moon, C., Cooper, R., & Fifer, W. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior & Development, 16, 495–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Nawrot, E. S. (2003). The perception of emotional expression in music: Evidence from infants, children and adults. Psychology of Music, 31(1), 75–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Noë, A. (2009). Conscious reference. The Philosophical Quarterly, 59(236), 470–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nonken, M. (2008). What do musical chairs afford? On Clarke’s ways of listening and Sacks’s musicophilia. Ecological Psychology, 20(4), 283–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. O’Callaghan, Casey, “Auditory Perception”, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2009 Edition), Zalta E. N. (Ed.), URL = <http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2009/entries/perception-auditory/>.
  33. Papousek, H. (1996). Musicality in infancy research: Biological and cultural origins of early musicality. In I. Deliege & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Musical beginnings (pp. 37–55). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., Bregman, M. R., & Schulz, I. (2009). Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Current Biology, 19(10), 827–830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Philips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. (2005). Feeling the beat: Movement influences rhythm perception. Science, 308, 1430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Philips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: Auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition, 105(3), 533–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  38. Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1996). Natural musical intervals. Psychological Science, 7(5), 272–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schutz, A. (1976). Making music together: A study in social relationship. In A. Broderson (Ed.), Collected papers (Vol. 2, pp. 159–178). The Hague: Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  40. Scruton, R. (1997). The aesthetics of music. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  41. Sloboda, J. A. (2005). Exploring the musical mind: Cognition, emotion, ability, function. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Small, C. (1998). Musicking. Middletown: Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Standley, J. (2001). Music therapy for the neonate. Newborn Infant Nursing Reviews, 1(4), 211–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Standley, J. (2002). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of music therapy for premature infants. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 17(2), 107–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Standley, J., & Madsen, C. (1990). Comparison of infant preferences and responses to auditory stimuli: Music, mother, and other female voice. Journal of Music Therapy, 27, 54–97.Google Scholar
  47. Stoffregen, T. A. (2003). Affordances as properties of the animal-environment system. Ecological Psychology, 15(2), 115–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Trainor, L. J., & Heinmiller, B. M. (1998). The development of evaluative responses to music: Infants prefer to listen to consonance over dissonance. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(1), 77–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Trehub, S. E. (2003). The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 669–673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Trehub, S. E., & Schellenberg, E. G. (1995). Music: Its relevance to infants. Annals of Child Development, 11, 1–24.Google Scholar
  51. Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, E. G., & Kamenetsky, S. (1999). Infants’ and adults’ perception of scale structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(4), 965.Google Scholar
  52. Trehub, S. E., & Trainor, L. (1993). Listening strategies in infancy: The roots of music and language development. In S. Mcadams & E. Bigand (Eds.), Thinking in sound: The cognitive psychology of human audition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Trehub, S. E., & Trainor, L. J. (1998). Singing to infants: Lullabies and play songs. Advances in Infancy Research, 12, 43–77.Google Scholar
  54. Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue on Rhythms, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication, 157–213.Google Scholar
  55. Trevarthen, C., & Malloch, S. N. (2000). The dance of wellbeing: Defining the musical therapeutic effect. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy, 9(2), 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Vickhoff, B., & Malmgren, H. (2004). Why does music move us? Philosophical Communications, Web series No 34. Sweden: Department of Philosophy, Göteborg University. ISSN 1652-0459.Google Scholar
  57. Wagner, R. (1994). Introductory musical acoustics. Raleigh: Contemporary Publishing.Google Scholar
  58. Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. Hillsdale: Earlbaum.Google Scholar
  59. Zillman, D., & Gan, S. (1997). Musical taste in adolescence. In D. Hargreaves & A. North (Eds.), The social psychology of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Zentner, M., & Kagan, J. (1998). Infants’ perception of consonance and dissonance in music. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(3), 483–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Danish National Research Foundation: Center for Subjectivity ResearchUniversity of CopenhagenCopenhagen SDenmark

Personalised recommendations