Abstract
Testing of verbal fluency is currently part of standard presurgical neuropsychological assessment for patients with focal epilepsy. However, to date no systematic review has been conducted on semantic (SVF) and phonemic verbal fluency (PVF) in this patient group. The present review compares verbal fluency between healthy control subjects and subgroups of adult presurgical patients with focal epilepsy according to lateralisation and localisation of the dysfunction. PubMed was searched with a comprehensive search string. Abstracts of all studies and full-texts of potentially relevant studies were screened. Study quality was assessed by independent raters according to predefined criteria. 39 studies were included. Meta-analyses were performed to compare SVF and PVF across groups of patients with temporal (TLE) and frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) as well as healthy controls (HC). Both patients with left- and right sided TLE were impaired on SVF and PVF compared to HC. Patients with left-sided TLE were slightly more impaired than patients with right-sided TLE. Patients with FLE showed a larger impairment in PVF than patients with TLE, whereas on SVF there was no difference between FLE and TLE. For TLE comparisons the study pool seems to have been sufficient, whereas more studies are needed to verify results for FLE. Semantic verbal fluency might not differentiate between FLE and TLE. While verbal fluency impairment was anticipated, especially in left-sided TLE and FLE patients, the impairment in patients with right-sided TLE was larger than expected. Results are discussed with regard to neuropsychological theory and practice.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Adcock, J. E., Wise, R. G., Oxbury, J. M., Oxbury, S. M., & Matthews, P. M. (2003). Quantitative fMRI assessment of the differences in lateralization of language-related brain activation in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. NeuroImage, 18, 423–438.
Aikia, M., Salmenpera, T., Partanen, K., & Kalviainen, R. (2001). Verbal Memory in Newly Diagnosed Patients and Patients with Chronic Left Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2, 20–27.
Alessio, A., Bonilha, L., Rorden, C., Kobayashi, E., Min, L. L., Damasceno, B. P., et al. (2006). Memory and language impairments and their relationships to hippocampal and perirhinal cortex damage in patients with medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8, 593–600.
Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 17–42.
Arnold, S., Schlaug, G., Niemann, H., Ebner, A., Luders, H., Witte, O. W., et al. (1996). Topography of interictal glucose hypometabolism in unilateral mesiotemporal epilepsy. Neurology, 46, 1422–1430.
Badawy, R. A., Johnson, K. A., Cook, M. J., & Harvey, A. S. (2012). A mechanistic appraisal of cognitive dysfunction in epilepsy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1885–1896.
Baker, G. A., Taylor, J., & Aldenkamp, A. P. (2011). Newly diagnosed epilepsy: cognitive outcome after 12 months. Epilepsia, 52, 1084–1091.
Baldo, J. V., Shimamura, A. P., Delis, D. C., Kramer, J., & Kaplan, E. (2001). Verbal and design fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 586–596.
Bartha, L., Benke, T., Bauer, G., & Trinka, E. (2005). Interictal language functions in temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76, 808–814.
Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. D., & Sivan, A. B. (1994). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: AJA Associates.
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2767–2796.
Bonelli, S. B., Powell, R., Thompson, P. J., Yogarajah, M., Focke, N. K., Stretton, J., et al. (2011). Hippocampal activation correlates with visual confrontation naming: fMRI findings in controls and patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 95, 246–254.
Brückner, K. (2012). Standard der neuropsychologischen Testung in der prächirurgischen Epilepsiediagnostik. Stellungnahme der neuropsychologischen DGfE-Kommission. Zeitschrift für Epileptologie, 25, 259–263.
Brückner, K., Lutz, M., & Arbeitskreis Epilepsie/Erwachsene. (2010). Stellungnahme des neuropsychologischen Arbeitskreises Epilepsie/Erwachsene zum Vorschlag der Arbeitsgemeinschaft für prächirurgische Epilepsiediagnostik und operative Epilepsietherapie für Standards in der neuropsychologischen prä- und postchirurgischen Epilepsiediagnostik. Zeitschrift für Epileptologie, 23, 64–65.
Cardebat, D., Doyon, B., Puel, M., Goulet, P., & Joanette, Y. (1990). Formal and semantic lexical evocation in normal subjects. Performance and dynamics of production as a function of sex, age and educational level. Acta Neurological Belgica, 90, 207–217.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Corcoran, R., & Upton, D. (1993). A role for the hippocampus in card sorting? Cortex, 29, 293–304.
Davies, K. G., Maxwell, R. E., Beniak, T. E., Destafney, E., & Fiol, M. E. (1995). Language function after temporal lobectomy without stimulation mapping of cortical function. Epilepsia, 36, 130–136.
Davies, K. G., Bell, B. D., Bush, A. J., Hermann, B. P., Dohan, F. C., Jr, Jaap, A. S. (1998). Naming decline after left anterior temporal lobectomy correlates with pathological status of resected hippocampus. Epilepsia, 39, 407--19.
Deelman, B. G., Liebrand, W. B., Koning-Haanstra, M., & van den Burg, W. (1980). Measurements of aphasic disorders: a brief description of the SAN-battery. Gerontologie, 11, 17–21.
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2011). Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
DerSimonian, R., & Laird, N. (1986). Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Controlled Clinical Trials, 7, 177–188.
Doucet, G. E., Skidmore, C., Sharan, A. D., Sperling, M. R., & Tracy, J. I. (2013). Functional connectivity abnormalities vary by amygdala subdivision and are associated with psychiatric symptoms in unilateral temporal epilepsy. Brain and Cognition, 83, 171–182.
Drane, D. L., Lee, G. P., Cech, H., Huthwaite, J. S., Ojemann, G. A., Ojemann, J. G., et al. (2006). Structured cueing on a semantic fluency task differentiates patients with temporal versus frontal lobe seizure onset. Epilepsy & Behavior, 9, 339–344.
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. E. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634.
Field, S. J., Saling, M. M., & Berkovic, S. F. (2000). Interictal discourse production in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain and Language, 74, 213–222.
Gardini, S., Cuetos, F., Fasano, F., Pellegrini, F. F., Marchi, M., Venneri, A., et al. (2013). Brain structural substrates of semantic memory decline in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Current Alzheimer Research, 10, 373–389.
Giovagnoli, A. R., & Avanzini, G. (1996). Forgetting rate and interference effects on a verbal memory distractor task in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 259–264.
Giovagnoli, A. R., & Avanzini, G. (2000). Quality of life and memory performance in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 101, 295–300.
Giovagnoli, A. R., & Bell, B. (2011). Drawing from memory in focal epilepsy: a cognitive and neural perspective. Epilepsy Research, 94, 69–74.
Giovagnoli, A. R., Canafoglia, L., Reati, F., Raviglione, F., & Franceschetti, S. (2009). The neuropsychological pattern of Unverricht-Lundborg disease. Epilepsy Research, 84, 217–223.
Giovannetti, T., Goldstein, R. Z., Schullery, M., Barr, W. B., & Bilder, R. M. (2003). Category fluency in first-episode schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 384–393.
Gleissner, U., & Elger, C. E. (2001). The hippocampal contribution to verbal fluency in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex, 37, 55–63.
Haneef, Z., Lenartowicz, A., Yeh, H. J., Engel, J., Jr., & Stern, J. M. (2012). Effect of lateralized temporal lobe epilepsy on the default mode network. Epilepsy and Behavior, 25, 350–357.
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. San Diego: Academic Press.
Helmstädter, C., & Elger, C. E. (2000). Behavioral markers for self- and other-attribution of memory: a study in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy and healthy volunteers. Epilepsy Research, 41, 235–243.
Helmstädter, C., Kurthen, M., Linke, D. B., & Elger, C. E. (1994). Right hemisphere restitution of language and memory functions in right hemisphere language-dominant patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain, 117(Pt 4), 729–737.
Helmstädter, C., Kemper, B., & Elger, C. E. (1996). Neuropsychological aspects of frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 34, 399–406.
Henry, J., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits in depression. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 78–101.
Hermann, B. P., & Wyler, A. R. (1988). Effects of anterior temporal lobectomy on language function: a controlled study. Annals of Neurology, 23, 585–588.
Hermann, B. P., Bell, B., Seidenberg, M., & Woodard, A. (2001). Learning disabilities and language function in epilepsy. Epilepsia, 42(Suppl 1), 21–23.
Higgins, J. P. & Green, S. e. (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration.
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altmann, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557–560.
Horn, W. (1983). L-P-S Leistungsprüfsystem (2nd ed.). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Howell, R. A., Saling, M. M., Bradley, D. C., & Berkovic, S. F. (1994). Interictal language fluency in temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex, 30, 469–478.
Isaacs, K. L., Barr, W. B., Nelson, P. K., & Devinsky, O. (2006). Degree of handedness and cerebral dominance. Neurology, 66, 1855–1858.
Joanette, Y., Ska, B., & Cote, H. (2004). Protocole MEC - Protocole Montréal d’Évaluation de la Communication. Montreal: Ortho Edition.
Jokeit, H., Heger, R., Ebner, A., & Markowitsch, H. J. (1998). Hemispheric asymmetries in category-specific word retrieval. Neuroreport, 9, 2371–2373.
Jones-Gotman, M., Smith, M. L., Risse, G. L., Westerveld, M., Swanson, S. J., Giovagnoli, A. R., et al. (2010). The contribution of neuropsychology to diagnostic assessment in epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 18, 3–12.
Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Floel, A., et al. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123(Pt 12), 2512–2518.
Laisney, M., Matuszewski, V., Mezenge, F., Belliard, S., de la Sayette, V., Eustache, F., et al. (2009). The underlying mechanisms of verbal fluency deficit in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Journal of Neurological, 256, 1083–1094.
Lee, S., Sziklas, V., Andermann, F., Farnham, S., Risse, G., Gustafson, M., et al. (2003). The effects of adjunctive topiramate on cognitive function in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 44, 339–347.
Lehericy, S., Cohen, L., Bazin, B., Samson, S., Giacomini, E., Rougetet, R., et al. (2000). Functional MR evaluation of temporal and frontal language dominance compared with the Wada test. Neurology, 54, 1625–1633.
Loiseau, J., Loiseau, P., Guyot, M., Duche, B., Dartigues, J. F., & Aublet, B. (1990). Survey of seizure disorders in the French southwest. I. Incidence of epileptic syndromes. Epilepsia, 31, 391–396.
Lutz, M. T., Clusmann, H., Elger, C. E., Schramm, J., & Helmstaedter, C. (2004). Neuropsychological outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy with transsylvian versus transcortical approach: a randomized prospective clinical trial of surgery for temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 45, 809–816.
MacDonald, B. K., Cockerell, O. C., Sander, J. W. A. S., & Shorvon, S. D. (2000). The incidence and lifetime prevalence of neurological disorders in a prospective community-based study in the UK. Brain, 123, 665–676.
Martin, R. C., Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (1990). The effects of lateralized temporal lobe dysfunction on formal and semantic word fluency. Neuropsychologia, 28, 823–829.
Martin, R. C., Sawrie, S. M., Edwards, R., Roth, D. L., Faught, E., Kuzniecky, R. I., et al. (2000). Investigation of executive function change following anterior temporal lobectomy: selective normalization of verbal fluency. Neuropsychology, 14, 501–508.
McDonald, C. R., Ahmadi, M. E., Hagler, D. J., Tecoma, E. S., Iragui, V. J., Gharapetian, L., et al. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of memory and language impairments in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology, 71, 1869–1876.
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altmann, D. G., & PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 62, 1006–1012.
N’Kaoua, B., Lespinet, V., Barsse, A., Rougier, A., & Claverie, B. (2001). Exploration of hemispheric specialization and lexico-semantic processing in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy with verbal fluency tasks. Neuropsychologia, 39, 635–642.
Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Vallar, G., & Cappa, S. F. (1986). Three clinical tests to research and rate the lexical performance of normal subjects. Archivio di psicologia, neurologia e psichiatria, 47, 477–506.
O’Shea, M. F., Saling, M. M., Bladin, P. F., & Berkovic, S. F. (1996). Does naming contribute to memory self-report in temporal lobe epilepsy? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 98–109.
Ortinski, P., & Meador, K. J. (2004). Cognitive side effects of antiepileptic drugs. Epilepsy and Behavior, 5(Suppl 1), S60–S65.
Pachlatko, C. (2008). Ökonomische Aspekte Informationszentrum Epilepsie (ize) der Dt. Gesellschaft für Epileptologie e.V.
Parks, R. W., Loewenstein, D. A., Dodrill, K. L., Barker, W. W., Yoshii, F., Chang, J. Y., et al. (1988). Cerebral metabolic effects of a verbal fluency test: a PET scan study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 565–575.
Piazzini, A., Turner, K., Vignoli, A., Canger, R., & Canevini, M. P. (2008). Frontal cognitive dysfunction in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia, 49, 657–662.
Pisano, T., Marini, C., Brovedani, P., Brizzolara, D., Pruna, D., Mei, D., et al. (2005). Abnormal phonologic processing in familial lateral temporal lobe epilepsy due to a new LGI1 mutation. Epilepsia, 46, 118–123.
Quigg, M., Broshek, D. K., Heidal-Schiltz, S., Maedgen, J. W., & Bertram, E. H., III. (2003). Depression in intractable partial epilepsy varies by laterality of focus and surgery. Epilepsia, 44, 419–424.
Ramirez, M. J., Schefft, B. K., Howe, S. R., Hovanitz, C., Yeh, H. S., & Privitera, M. D. (2010). The effects of perceived emotional distress on language performance in intractable epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav.
Risse, G. L. (2006). Cognitive outcomes in patients with frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 47(Suppl 2), 87–89.
Saykin, A. J., Stafiniak, P., Robinson, L. J., Flannery, K. A., Gur, R. C., O’Connor, M. J., et al. (1995). Language before and after temporal lobectomy: specificity of acute changes and relation to early risk factors. Epilepsia, 36, 1071–1077.
Sheldon, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2012). The Nature and Time-Course of Medial Temporal Lobe Contributions to Semantic Retrieval: An fMRI Study on Verbal Fluency. Hippocampus, 22, 1451–1466.
Sherman, E. M., Wiebe, S., Fay-McClymont, T. B., Tellez-Zenteno, J., Metcalfe, A., Hernandez-Ronquillo, L., et al. (2011). Neuropsychological outcomes after epilepsy surgery: systematic review and pooled estimates. Epilepsia, 52, 857–869.
Siegel, A. M. (2004). Presurgical evaluation and surgical treatment of medically refractory epilepsy. Neurosurgical Review, 27, 1–18.
Stroup, D. F., Berlin, J. A., Morton, S. C., Olkin, I., Williamson, G. D., Rennie, D., et al. (2000). Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA, 283, 2008–2012.
Suchy, Y., Sands, K., & Chelune, G. J. (2003). Verbal and nonverbal fluency performance before and after seizure surgery. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 190–200.
Thivard, L., Hombrouck, J., du Montcel, S. T., Delmaire, C., Cohen, L., Samson, S., et al. (2005). Productive and perceptive language reorganization in temporal lobe epilepsy. NeuroImage, 24, 841–851.
Trebuchon-Da Fonseca, A., Guedj, E., Alario, F. X., Laguitton, V., Mundler, O., Chauvel, P., et al. (2009). Brain regions underlying word finding difficulties in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain, 132, 2772–2784.
Tröster, A. I., Warmflash, V., Osorio, I., Paolo, A. M., Alexander, L. J., & Barr, W. B. (1995). The roles of semantic networks and search efficiency in verbal fluency performance in intractable temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 21, 19--26.
Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 370–378.
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: the effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36, 499–504.
Tudesco, I. S., Vaz, L. J., Mantoan, M. A., Belzunces, E., Noffs, M. H., Caboclo, L. O., et al. (2010). Assessment of working memory in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy associated with unilateral hippocampal sclerosis. Epilepsy and Behavior, 18, 223–228.
Upton, D., & Thompson, P. J. (1996). General neuropsychological characteristics of frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 23, 169–177.
Verma, M., & Howard, R. J. (2012). Semantic memory and language dysfunction in early Alzheimer’s disease: a review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 1209–1217.
Vermeulen, J., & Aldenkamp, A. P. (1995). Cognitive side-effects of chronic antiepileptic drug treatment: a review of 25 years of research. Epilepsy Research, 22, 65–95.
Vivas, A. C., Baaj, A. A., Benbadis, S. R., & Vale, F. L. (2012). The health care burden of patients with epilepsy in the United States: an analysis of a nationwide database over 15 years. Neurosurgical Focus, 32, E1.
Vlooswijk, M. C., Jansen, J. F., Majoie, H. J., Hofman, P. A., de Krom, M. C., Aldenkamp, A. P., et al. (2010). Functional connectivity and language impairment in cryptogenic localization-related epilepsy. Neurology, 75, 395–402.
Voets, N. L., Adcock, J. E., Flitney, D. E., Behrens, T. E., Hart, Y., Stacey, R., et al. (2006). Distinct right frontal lobe activation in language processing following left hemisphere injury. Brain, 129, 754–766.
Wang, X. Q., Lang, S. Y., Hong, L. U., Lin, M. A., Yan-ling, M. A., & Yang, F. (2010). Changes in extratemporal integrity and cognition in temporal lobe epilepsy: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurology India, 58, 891–899.
Wiebe, S., Blume, W. T., Girvin, J. P., & Eliasziw, M. (2001). Effectiveness and Efficiency of Surgery for Temporal Lobe Epilepsy Study Group. A randomized, controlled trial of surgery for temporal-lobe epilepsy. The New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 311–318.
Yang, J. (2014). The role of the right hemisphere in metaphor comprehension: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 107–122.
Conflict of interest
None.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Metternich and Buschmann, both authors contributed equally to this work.
Appendix
Appendix
Details on study selection
The abstracts of all identified studies were screened by two independent raters according to predefined criteria (see Table 1). Initially, in 99.6 % raters agreed on whether a study was to be included or excluded. The single case of dissent was resolved by discussion. In 80 % there was initial agreement on the exact reasons for exclusion that could be drawn from the abstract, i.e. mostly the number of exclusion reasons provided differed between raters. Disagreements were resolved by discussion. Full-texts of the studies identified as potentially relevant in the first screening were examined for inclusion by the same independent raters. If important data were missing or insufficiently reported, the corresponding author of the publication was contacted, and in case of a response the provided information was used in the review. Studies judged to be eligible after examination of full-texts represent the study population included in the present review.
Details on assessment of risk of bias
Regarding comparability, the tolerated difference between groups on demographic and clinical variables was half a pooled standard deviation. If data were missing or not reported, this resulted in a negative rating. We weighted the categories listed in the table below according to their considered importance as sources of bias. The quality assessment per criterion was multiplied with the category weight (see table). The resulting figures for each criterion were added up; studies with a sum score of ≥ 1 were considered to bear a low risk of bias. All other studies were classed as being at high risk.
Categories rated in the risk of bias assessment and their assigned weights:
Category | Comparability of groups on IQ/education, duration/onset of epilepsy, age | Comparability of groups on seizure frequency | Comparability of groups on AED | Language dominance measures | Sample size | Recruitment | Blinded assessment of verbal fluency | Quality of outcome measures |
Weight | 2 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0,5 | 0,5 | 0,5 |
Scores of −2 to +2 were given for each category and multiplied with the category weight (detailed table available from the authors).
Example: Adcock et al. 2003 (category rating x category weight):
Sum scores below +1 were classed as high risk of bias.
Details on data synthesis
Using the standardized mean difference as effect measure was necessary because the primary studies assessed the same outcome with several different measures. If not reported by the primary authors, standard deviations were calculated according to the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins & Green, 2011). Due to expected clinical and methodological diversity across studies, individual effect sizes were summarized using a random effects model, which assumes that each study estimates different effects that follow a common distribution (DerSimonian and Laird 1986). The subgroup analysis in studies including patients with solely mesiotemporal pathology was pre-specified. Studies reporting data for individual patients were also included here. In these cases we manually extracted the relevant data for patients with mesiotemporal pathology. In order to determine whether the main results were influenced by sources of bias, we conducted a pre-specified subgroup analysis for major comparisons including only studies at low risk of bias. In addition, as one might consider the presence of a healthy control group in a study as a proxy indicator for study quality, we conducted a subgroup analysis for comparisons between diseased groups including only studies with healthy control groups. This analysis followed expert recommendation during the peer-review process and was therefore post hoc. Funnel plots were investigated for comparisons including at least ten studies. Funnel plots are scatter plots displaying results of individual studies along with their precision. If no publication bias is present, this plot should resemble a symmetrical inverted funnel. All analyses were performed using Review Manager 5.1 (Cochrane Collaboration, Copenhagen).
Additional materials, e.g., tables for risk of bias assessment/analyses and forest plots are available from the authors.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Metternich, B., Buschmann, F., Wagner, K. et al. Verbal Fluency in Focal Epilepsy: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 24, 200–218 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9255-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-014-9255-8