Skip to main content
Log in

The Riddle of Frontal Lobe Function in Man

  • Published:
Neuropsychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14
Fig. 15

References

  • Ach, N. (1905). Über die Willenstatigkeit und das Denken (pp. 10–294). Göttingen: Vandenhocck & Ruprecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerly, S. S. (1935). Instinctive, emotional and mental changes following pre-frontal lobe extirpation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 92, 717–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajuriaguerra, J. de, & Hécaen, H. (1960). Le Cortex cérébral (p. 458). Paris: Masson et Cie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aubert, H. (1861). Eine scheinbare bedeutende Drehung von Objekten bei Neigung des Kopfes nach rechts oder links. Virchows Archiv fuer Pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie, 20, 381–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Battersby, W. S. (1951). The regional gradient of critical flicker frequency after frontal or occipital lobe injury. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 59–68.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Battersby, W. S., Bender, M. B., & Teuber, H.-L. (1951). Effects of total light flux on critical flicker frequency after frontal lobe lesion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 135–142.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Battersby, W. S., Krieger, H. P., & Bender, M. B. (1955). Visual and tactile discriminative learning in patients with cerebral tumors. American Journal of Psychology, 68, 562–574.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Battersby, W. S., Teuber, H.-L., & Bender, M. B. (1953). Problem-solving behavior in men with frontal or occipital brain injuries. Journal of Psychology, 35, 329–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bender, M., & Jung, R. (1948). Abweichunge der subjektiven optischen Vertikalen und Horizontalen bei Gesunden und Hirnverletzten. Arch. Psychiat. Z. Neural., 181, 193–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beringer, K. (1941). Über Störungen des Antriebes bei einem von der unteren Falxkante ausgehenden doppelseitigen Meningeom. Zeitschrift für die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 171, 451–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi, L. (1895). The functions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 18, 497–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blum, R. A. (1952). Effects of subtotal lesions of frontal granular cortex on delayed reaction in monkeys. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 67, 375–386.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brickner, R. M. (1934). An interpretation of frontal lobe function based upon the study of a case of partial bilateral frontal lobectomy. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 13, 259–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickner, R. M. (1936). The intellectual functions of the frontal lobes: A study based upon observation of a man after partial bilateral frontal lobectomy. New York: The Macmillan Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickner, R. M. (1939). Bilateral frontal lobectomy: Follow-up report of a case. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 41, 580–583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickner, R. M. (1952). Brain of patient “A” after bilateral frontal lobectomy; status of frontal lobe problem. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 68, 293–313.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brodal, A., Szabo, T., & Torvik, A. (1956). Cortico-fugal fibres to sensory trigeminal nuclei and nucleus of solitary tract. An experimental study in the cat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 106, 527–555.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. G. (1922). Reflex orientation of the optical axes and the influence upon it of the cerebral cortex. Archives Néerlandaises de Physiologie, 7, 571–578.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruns, L. (1892). Über Störungen des Gleichgewichts bei Stirnhirntumor. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift, 18, 138–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchwald, N. A., Wyers, E. J., Heuser, G., Rakie, L., & Hull, C. (1962). Visual integration and the “caudate loop”. Experimental Neurology, 5, 1–20.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, H. A. (1913). cited by Hunter, W. S.

  • Cohen, L. (1959). Perception of reversible figures after brain injury. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 81, 765–775.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, K. (1932). Versuch einer psychologischen Analyse des Parietalsyndroms. Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 84, 28–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, M. (1930). The anterior cerebral artery and its syndromes. Brain, 53, 120–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. Baltimore: The Williams & Wilkins Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delmas-Marsalat, P. (1936). Lobe frontal et équilibre. Encéphale, 31, 15–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny-Brown, D. (1951). The frontal lobes and their functions. Chap. 2. In A. Feiling (Ed.), Modern trends in neurology (pp. 13–89). New York: Paul B. Hoeber, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny-Brown, D. (1962). The basal ganglia and their relation to disorders of movement. In W. R. Russell & M. L. E. Espir (Eds.), Oxford Neurol. Monog. (p. 144). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuchtwanger, E. (1923). Die Funktionen des Stirnhirns, ihre Pathologie und Psychologie. In O. Foerster & K. Williams (Eds.), Monogr. aus Gesget. Neurol. Psychiat., Heft 38 (pp. 4–194). Berlin: Springer-Verlag OHG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelb, A., & Goldstein, K. (1920). Psychologische Analysen hirnpathologischer Fälle. Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerstmann, J. (1916). Zur Kenntnis der Störungen des Köpergleichgewichtes nach Schussverletzungen des Stirnhirns. Msch. Psychiatr. Neurol., 40, 354–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerstmann, J. (1924). Fingeragnosie: Eine umschriebene Störung der Orientierung am eigenen Körper. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 37, 1010–1012.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerstmann, J. (1930). Zur Symptomatologie der Hirnläsionen im Übergangsgebiet der uteren Parietal-und mittleren Occipitalwindung. (Das Syndrom: Fingeragnosic, Rechts-Links-Storung, Agraphie, Akalkulie). Nervenarzt, 3, 691–695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghent, L. (1956). Perception of overlapping and embedded figures by children of different ages. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 567–587.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghent, L., Mishkin, M., & Teuber, H.-L. (1962). Short-term memory after frontal-lobe injury in man. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 55, 705–709.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K. (1931). Uber Zeigen und Greifen. Nervenarzt, 4, 453–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K. (1936). The significance of the frontal lobes for mental performances. Journal of Neurology and Psychopathology, 17, 27–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K., & Katz, S. E. (1937). The psychopathology of Pick’s disease. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 38, 473–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K., & Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete behavior: An experimental study with special tests. Psychol. Monogr., 53, No. 2 (whole no. 239). Evanston, IL: Amer. Psychol. Assoc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halstead, W. C. (1947). Brain and Intelligence: A quantitative study of the frontal lobes. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrower, M. R. (1939). Changes in figure-ground perception in patients with cortical lesions. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 30, 47–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1939). Intelligence in man after large removals of cerebral tissue: Report of four left frontal lobe cases. Journal of General Psychology, 21, 73–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1945). Man’s frontal lobes: A critical review. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 54, 421–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O., & Penfield, W. (1940). Human behavior after extensive bilateral removals from the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 44, 421–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Held, R. (1961). Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 32, 26–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holst, E. v., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwichen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, W. S. (1913–1915). The delayed reaction in animals and children. Behav. Monogr., 2(1), 1–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jabbur, S. J., & Towe, A. L. (1961). Cortical excitation of neurons in dorsal column nuclei of cat, including an analysis of pathways. Journal of Neurophysiology, 24, 499–509.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobsen, C. F. (1935). Functions of the frontal association area in primates. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 33, 558–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janischewsky, A. (1909). Un cas de maladie de Parkinson. Rev. Neurol., 17, 823–831.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennard, M. A. (1939). Alterations in response to visual stimuli following lesions in the frontal lobes of monkeys. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, Chicago, 41, 1153–1165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennard, M. A., & Ectors, L. (1938). Forced circling in monkeys following lesions of the frontal lobes. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1, 45–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. (1933). Ueber die Funktionen des Parietallappens. Nervenarzt, 6(1–7), 67–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleist, K. (1908). Untersuchungen zur Kenntnis der psychomotorischen Bewegungsstörungen bei Geisteskranken (p. 179). Leipzig: Klinkhardt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleist, K. (1931). Gehirnpathologische und lokalisatoriche Ergebnisse: Die Störungen der Ichleistungen und ihre Lokalisation im Orbital-, Innen-und Zwichenhirn. Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 79, 338–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleist, K. (1934). Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, I. (1951). Ueber Aufbau und Wadlungen der Wahrnehmungswelt. Sitzungsberichte - Österreichische Akademie der Wissenschaften – Historische Klasse, 227, 1–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1960). Central conical projections to motor and somato-sensory cell groups. Brain, 83, 161–184.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawicka, W., & Konorski, J. (1959). The physiological mechanism of delayed reactions. III. The effects of prefrontal ablations on delayed reactions in dogs. Acta Biologiae Experimentalis (Warsaw), 19, 221–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lhermitte, J. (1939). L’Image de notre corps. Paris: Edit. Novu. Rev. Crit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeb, J. (1884). Die Sehstörungen nach Verletzung der Grosshirnrinde. Pflügers Archiv für die Gesamte Physiologie, 34, 67–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magni, F., Melzack, R., Moruzzi, G., & Smith, C. J. (1959). Direct pyramidal influences on the dorsal-column nuclei. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 97, 357–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning in humans: II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, 181–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, N. R. F. (1945). Reasoning in humans: III. The mechanism of equivalent stimuli and of reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 349–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettler, F. A. (Ed.) (Columbia-Greystone Associates) (1949). Selective partial ablation of the frontal cortex. A correlative study of its effects on human psychotic subjects (XIV & 517). New York: Paul B. Hoeber, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettler, F. A. (by the Columbia-Greystone Associates. 2d Group) (1952). Psychosurgical problems. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., Blakiston Division.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. R., Harlow, H. F., & Settlage, P. H. (1951). A survey of delayed response performance by normal and brain-damaged monkeys. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 44, 17–25.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mishkin, M., Prockop, E. S., & Rosvold, H. E. (1962). One-trial object-discrimination learning in monkeys with frontal lesions. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 55, 178–181.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Muskens, L. J. J. (1934). Das Supra-vestibulare system (XVIII and 557). Amsterdam: Noord-Hollandsche Uitgeversmaatschappij.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, J. M. (1938). Gertsmann syndrome: Finger agnosia, agraphia, confusion of right and left and acalculia. Comparison of this syndrome with disturbance of body scheme resulting from lesions of the right side of the brain. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 39, 536–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partridge, M. (1950). Pre-frontal leucotomy. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas, Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrie, A. (1952). Personality and the frontal lobes. London: Routledge & Kegan Patil, Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, M. (1955). Effect of brain tumor on perception of hidden figures, sorting behavior, and problem solving performance. Ph.D. Dissertation, New York University (Dissertation Abstr., 1960, 20, no. 8.)

  • Poppelreuter, W. (1917). Die psychischen Schädigugen durch Kopfschuss im Kriege 1914–16 (XVI and 210). Leipzig: Voss.

    Google Scholar 

  • Probst, M. (1903). Ueber die anatomischen und physiologischen Folgen der Halbseitendurchschneidung des Mittelhirns. Jb. Psychiat. Neurol., 24, 219–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, F. (1961). Sensorimotor status of Parkinsonian patients before and after surgical interference with subcortical structures. Ph.D. Dissertation, New York University.

  • Richter, C. P., & Hines, M. (1934). The production of the “grasp reflex” in adult macaques by experimental frontal lobe lesions. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 13, 211–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosvold, H. E., & Mishkin, M. (1950). Evaluation of the effects of prefrontal lobotomy on intelligence. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 4, 122–126.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rushworth, G., & Denny-Brown, D. (1959). The two components of the grasp reflex after ablation of frontal cortex in monkeys. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 51, 91–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, W. R. (1959). Brain, memory and learning. Fair Lawn, NJ: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rylander, G. (1939). Personality changes after operations on the frontal lobes: A clinical study of thirty-two cases. Acta Psychiat. Neurol., Copenhagen, Suppl. 20.

  • Rylander, G. (1943). Mental changes after excision of cerebral tissue. A clinical study of 16 cases of resections in the parietal, temporal and occipital lobes. Acta Psychiat. Neurol., Coppenhagen, Suppl. 25, 80.

  • Schuster, P., & Pinéas, H. (1926). Weitere Beobachtungen über Zwangsgreifen und Nachgreifen. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde, 91, 16–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1954). Performance on complex tactual tasks after brain injury in man: Analyses by locus of lesion. American Journal of Psychology, 67, 220–240.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1955). Spatial orientation in man after cerebral injury: I. Analyses by locus of lesion. Journal of Psychology, 39, 227–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1960). Somato-sensory changes after penetrating brain wounds in man. Commonwealth Fund. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1963). Impaired orientation in personal and extrapersonal space. Brain, 86 (in press).

  • Silberpfennig, J. (1941). Contributions to the problem of eye movements: III. Disturbances of ocular movements with pseudohemianopsia in frontal lobe tumors. Confinia Neurologica, 4, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 43, 482–489.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L. (1955). Physiological psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 6, 267–296.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L. (1959). Some alterations in behavior after cerebral lesions in man. In A. D. Bass (Ed.), Evolution of nervous control from primitive organisms in man (pp. 157–194). Washington, DC: Amer. Ass. Adv. Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L. (1960). Perception. Chap. LXV. In J. Field, H. W. Magcun, & V. E. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of Physiology, Section 1. Neurophysiology (Vol. 3). Washington, DC: Am. Physiol. Soc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L. (1961a). Sensory deprivation, sensory suppression and agnosia: Notes for a neurologic theory. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 132, 32–40.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L. (1961b). Brain and behavior (summation). In M. A. B. Brazier (Ed.), Int. Conf. on Brain and Behavior (pp. 393–420). Washington, DC: Amer. Inst. Biol. Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1949). Changes in visual searching performance following cerebral lesions. American Journal of Physiology, 159, 592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1951). Performance of complex visual tasks after cerebral lesions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 114, 413–429.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1960). Visual field defects after penetrating missile wounds of the brain. Commonwealth Fund. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Bender, M. B. (1948). Critical flicker frequency in defective fields of vision. Fed. Proc. Amer. Soc. Exp. Biol., 7, 123–124.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Bender, M. B. (1949). Alterations in pattern vision following trauma of occipital lobes in man. Journal of General Psychology, 40, 37–57.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Liebert, R. S. (1958). Specific and general effects of brain injury in man. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, Chicago, 80, 403–407.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Mishkin, M. (1954). Judgment of visual and postural vertical after brain injury. Journal of Psychology, 38, 161–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Rudel, R. G. (1962). Behaviour after cerebral lesions in children and adults. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 4, 3–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Weinstein, S. (1954). Performance on a formboard-task after penetrating brain injury. Journal of Psychology, 38, 177–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Weinstein, S. (1956). Ability to discover hidden figures after cerebral lesions. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, Chicago, 76, 369–379.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L., & Weinstein, S. (1958). Equipotentiality versus cortical localization. Science, 127, 241–242.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Towe, A., & Jabbur, S. J. (1961). Cortical inhibition of neurons in dorsal column nuclei of cat. Journal of Neurophysiology, 24, 488–489.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Monakow, C. (1910). Der rote Kern, die Haube und die Regio hypothalamica bei einigen Säugetieren und beim Menschen. Arbeiten aus dem Hirnanatomischen Institut in Zürich, 4, 103–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walberg, F. (1957). Corticofugal fibres to the nuclei of the dorsal columns. An experimental study in the cat. Brain, 80, 273–287.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weigl, E. (1941). On the psychology of so-called processes of abstraction. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36, 3–33 (transl. by Margaret J. Rioch from the original: “Zur Psychologie sogenannter Abstraktionsprozesse,” Z. Psychol., 1927, 103, 2–45).

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, S., Semmes, J., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1956). Spatial orientation in man after cerebral injury; II. Analyses according to concomitant defects. Journal of Psychology, 42, 249–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, S., & Teuber, H.-L. (1957). Effects of penetrating brain injury on intelligence test scores. Science, 125, 1036–1037.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, S., Teuber, H.-L., Ghent, L., & Semmes, J. (1955). Complex visual task performance after penetrating brain injury in man. American Psychologist, 10, 408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch, K., & Stuteville, P. (1958). Experimental production of unilateral neglect in monkeys. Brain, 81, 341–347.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex (p. 72). Max Cohn and Weigert: Breslau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, C. (1894–1896). Grundiss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen. Leipzig: Thieme.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yacorzynski, G. K., Boshes, B., & Davis, L. (1948). Psychological changes produced by frontal lobotomy. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 27, 642–657.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zangwill, O. L. (1951). Discussion on parietal lobe syndromes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine (Sect. Neurol.), 44, 343–346.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Additional information

This article has been re-typeset for clarity but the content is unchanged from the original version published as Teuber, HL. The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In JM Warren and K Akert. (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw Hill, pp. 410–477 (1964). Copyright 1964 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

1The program of research on which this study is based is carried out at the Psychophysiological Laboratory of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. The laboratory receives support from the Rockefeller and Hanford Foundations, and from the National Institutes of Health (under program grant M-S673). Additional aid is being received from the Life Sciences and Behavioral Sciences Programs of the Office of Scientific Research, U.S. Air Force, and from the Behavioral Biology Program of the National Aeronautics and Space Administration, under NASA grant NSG-496. In its earlier phases while at the Bellevue Medical Center in New York, from 1948 to 1962, the laboratory has been supported by the Commonwealth Fund of New York, by the U.S. Veterans Administration, and by the Office of the Surgeon General, U.S. Army, under Contract DA-49-007-MD-312.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Teuber, HL. The Riddle of Frontal Lobe Function in Man. Neuropsychol Rev 19, 25–46 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9088-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9088-z

Navigation