References
Ach, N. (1905). Über die Willenstatigkeit und das Denken (pp. 10–294). Göttingen: Vandenhocck & Ruprecht.
Ackerly, S. S. (1935). Instinctive, emotional and mental changes following pre-frontal lobe extirpation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 92, 717–729.
Ajuriaguerra, J. de, & Hécaen, H. (1960). Le Cortex cérébral (p. 458). Paris: Masson et Cie.
Aubert, H. (1861). Eine scheinbare bedeutende Drehung von Objekten bei Neigung des Kopfes nach rechts oder links. Virchows Archiv fuer Pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie, 20, 381–393.
Battersby, W. S. (1951). The regional gradient of critical flicker frequency after frontal or occipital lobe injury. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 59–68.
Battersby, W. S., Bender, M. B., & Teuber, H.-L. (1951). Effects of total light flux on critical flicker frequency after frontal lobe lesion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 135–142.
Battersby, W. S., Krieger, H. P., & Bender, M. B. (1955). Visual and tactile discriminative learning in patients with cerebral tumors. American Journal of Psychology, 68, 562–574.
Battersby, W. S., Teuber, H.-L., & Bender, M. B. (1953). Problem-solving behavior in men with frontal or occipital brain injuries. Journal of Psychology, 35, 329–351.
Bender, M., & Jung, R. (1948). Abweichunge der subjektiven optischen Vertikalen und Horizontalen bei Gesunden und Hirnverletzten. Arch. Psychiat. Z. Neural., 181, 193–212.
Beringer, K. (1941). Über Störungen des Antriebes bei einem von der unteren Falxkante ausgehenden doppelseitigen Meningeom. Zeitschrift für die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 171, 451–474.
Bianchi, L. (1895). The functions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 18, 497–530.
Blum, R. A. (1952). Effects of subtotal lesions of frontal granular cortex on delayed reaction in monkeys. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 67, 375–386.
Brickner, R. M. (1934). An interpretation of frontal lobe function based upon the study of a case of partial bilateral frontal lobectomy. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 13, 259–351.
Brickner, R. M. (1936). The intellectual functions of the frontal lobes: A study based upon observation of a man after partial bilateral frontal lobectomy. New York: The Macmillan Company.
Brickner, R. M. (1939). Bilateral frontal lobectomy: Follow-up report of a case. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 41, 580–583.
Brickner, R. M. (1952). Brain of patient “A” after bilateral frontal lobectomy; status of frontal lobe problem. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 68, 293–313.
Brodal, A., Szabo, T., & Torvik, A. (1956). Cortico-fugal fibres to sensory trigeminal nuclei and nucleus of solitary tract. An experimental study in the cat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 106, 527–555.
Brown, T. G. (1922). Reflex orientation of the optical axes and the influence upon it of the cerebral cortex. Archives Néerlandaises de Physiologie, 7, 571–578.
Bruns, L. (1892). Über Störungen des Gleichgewichts bei Stirnhirntumor. Deutsche Medizinische Wochenschrift, 18, 138–140.
Buchwald, N. A., Wyers, E. J., Heuser, G., Rakie, L., & Hull, C. (1962). Visual integration and the “caudate loop”. Experimental Neurology, 5, 1–20.
Carr, H. A. (1913). cited by Hunter, W. S.
Cohen, L. (1959). Perception of reversible figures after brain injury. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 81, 765–775.
Conrad, K. (1932). Versuch einer psychologischen Analyse des Parietalsyndroms. Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 84, 28–97.
Critchley, M. (1930). The anterior cerebral artery and its syndromes. Brain, 53, 120–165.
Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. Baltimore: The Williams & Wilkins Company.
Delmas-Marsalat, P. (1936). Lobe frontal et équilibre. Encéphale, 31, 15–91.
Denny-Brown, D. (1951). The frontal lobes and their functions. Chap. 2. In A. Feiling (Ed.), Modern trends in neurology (pp. 13–89). New York: Paul B. Hoeber, Inc.
Denny-Brown, D. (1962). The basal ganglia and their relation to disorders of movement. In W. R. Russell & M. L. E. Espir (Eds.), Oxford Neurol. Monog. (p. 144). London: Oxford University Press.
Feuchtwanger, E. (1923). Die Funktionen des Stirnhirns, ihre Pathologie und Psychologie. In O. Foerster & K. Williams (Eds.), Monogr. aus Gesget. Neurol. Psychiat., Heft 38 (pp. 4–194). Berlin: Springer-Verlag OHG.
Gelb, A., & Goldstein, K. (1920). Psychologische Analysen hirnpathologischer Fälle. Leipzig: Barth.
Gerstmann, J. (1916). Zur Kenntnis der Störungen des Köpergleichgewichtes nach Schussverletzungen des Stirnhirns. Msch. Psychiatr. Neurol., 40, 354–377.
Gerstmann, J. (1924). Fingeragnosie: Eine umschriebene Störung der Orientierung am eigenen Körper. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 37, 1010–1012.
Gerstmann, J. (1930). Zur Symptomatologie der Hirnläsionen im Übergangsgebiet der uteren Parietal-und mittleren Occipitalwindung. (Das Syndrom: Fingeragnosic, Rechts-Links-Storung, Agraphie, Akalkulie). Nervenarzt, 3, 691–695.
Ghent, L. (1956). Perception of overlapping and embedded figures by children of different ages. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 567–587.
Ghent, L., Mishkin, M., & Teuber, H.-L. (1962). Short-term memory after frontal-lobe injury in man. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 55, 705–709.
Goldstein, K. (1931). Uber Zeigen und Greifen. Nervenarzt, 4, 453–466.
Goldstein, K. (1936). The significance of the frontal lobes for mental performances. Journal of Neurology and Psychopathology, 17, 27–40.
Goldstein, K., & Katz, S. E. (1937). The psychopathology of Pick’s disease. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 38, 473–490.
Goldstein, K., & Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete behavior: An experimental study with special tests. Psychol. Monogr., 53, No. 2 (whole no. 239). Evanston, IL: Amer. Psychol. Assoc.
Grant, D. A., & Berg, E. A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404–411.
Halstead, W. C. (1947). Brain and Intelligence: A quantitative study of the frontal lobes. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Harrower, M. R. (1939). Changes in figure-ground perception in patients with cortical lesions. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 30, 47–51.
Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. London: Cambridge University Press.
Hebb, D. O. (1939). Intelligence in man after large removals of cerebral tissue: Report of four left frontal lobe cases. Journal of General Psychology, 21, 73–87.
Hebb, D. O. (1945). Man’s frontal lobes: A critical review. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 54, 421–438.
Hebb, D. O., & Penfield, W. (1940). Human behavior after extensive bilateral removals from the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 44, 421–438.
Held, R. (1961). Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 32, 26–32.
Holst, E. v., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwichen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.
Hunter, W. S. (1913–1915). The delayed reaction in animals and children. Behav. Monogr., 2(1), 1–86.
Jabbur, S. J., & Towe, A. L. (1961). Cortical excitation of neurons in dorsal column nuclei of cat, including an analysis of pathways. Journal of Neurophysiology, 24, 499–509.
Jacobsen, C. F. (1935). Functions of the frontal association area in primates. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 33, 558–569.
Janischewsky, A. (1909). Un cas de maladie de Parkinson. Rev. Neurol., 17, 823–831.
Kennard, M. A. (1939). Alterations in response to visual stimuli following lesions in the frontal lobes of monkeys. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, Chicago, 41, 1153–1165.
Kennard, M. A., & Ectors, L. (1938). Forced circling in monkeys following lesions of the frontal lobes. Journal of Neurophysiology, 1, 45–54.
Klein, R. (1933). Ueber die Funktionen des Parietallappens. Nervenarzt, 6(1–7), 67–74.
Kleist, K. (1908). Untersuchungen zur Kenntnis der psychomotorischen Bewegungsstörungen bei Geisteskranken (p. 179). Leipzig: Klinkhardt.
Kleist, K. (1931). Gehirnpathologische und lokalisatoriche Ergebnisse: Die Störungen der Ichleistungen und ihre Lokalisation im Orbital-, Innen-und Zwichenhirn. Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol., 79, 338–350.
Kleist, K. (1934). Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig: Barth.
Kohler, I. (1951). Ueber Aufbau und Wadlungen der Wahrnehmungswelt. Sitzungsberichte - Österreichische Akademie der Wissenschaften – Historische Klasse, 227, 1–118.
Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1960). Central conical projections to motor and somato-sensory cell groups. Brain, 83, 161–184.
Lawicka, W., & Konorski, J. (1959). The physiological mechanism of delayed reactions. III. The effects of prefrontal ablations on delayed reactions in dogs. Acta Biologiae Experimentalis (Warsaw), 19, 221–231.
Lhermitte, J. (1939). L’Image de notre corps. Paris: Edit. Novu. Rev. Crit.
Loeb, J. (1884). Die Sehstörungen nach Verletzung der Grosshirnrinde. Pflügers Archiv für die Gesamte Physiologie, 34, 67–172.
Magni, F., Melzack, R., Moruzzi, G., & Smith, C. J. (1959). Direct pyramidal influences on the dorsal-column nuclei. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 97, 357–377.
Maier, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning in humans: II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, 181–194.
Maier, N. R. F. (1945). Reasoning in humans: III. The mechanism of equivalent stimuli and of reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 349–360.
Mettler, F. A. (Ed.) (Columbia-Greystone Associates) (1949). Selective partial ablation of the frontal cortex. A correlative study of its effects on human psychotic subjects (XIV & 517). New York: Paul B. Hoeber, Inc.
Mettler, F. A. (by the Columbia-Greystone Associates. 2d Group) (1952). Psychosurgical problems. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., Blakiston Division.
Meyer, D. R., Harlow, H. F., & Settlage, P. H. (1951). A survey of delayed response performance by normal and brain-damaged monkeys. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 44, 17–25.
Mishkin, M., Prockop, E. S., & Rosvold, H. E. (1962). One-trial object-discrimination learning in monkeys with frontal lesions. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 55, 178–181.
Muskens, L. J. J. (1934). Das Supra-vestibulare system (XVIII and 557). Amsterdam: Noord-Hollandsche Uitgeversmaatschappij.
Nielsen, J. M. (1938). Gertsmann syndrome: Finger agnosia, agraphia, confusion of right and left and acalculia. Comparison of this syndrome with disturbance of body scheme resulting from lesions of the right side of the brain. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 39, 536–560.
Partridge, M. (1950). Pre-frontal leucotomy. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas, Publisher.
Petrie, A. (1952). Personality and the frontal lobes. London: Routledge & Kegan Patil, Ltd.
Pollack, M. (1955). Effect of brain tumor on perception of hidden figures, sorting behavior, and problem solving performance. Ph.D. Dissertation, New York University (Dissertation Abstr., 1960, 20, no. 8.)
Poppelreuter, W. (1917). Die psychischen Schädigugen durch Kopfschuss im Kriege 1914–16 (XVI and 210). Leipzig: Voss.
Probst, M. (1903). Ueber die anatomischen und physiologischen Folgen der Halbseitendurchschneidung des Mittelhirns. Jb. Psychiat. Neurol., 24, 219–325.
Proctor, F. (1961). Sensorimotor status of Parkinsonian patients before and after surgical interference with subcortical structures. Ph.D. Dissertation, New York University.
Richter, C. P., & Hines, M. (1934). The production of the “grasp reflex” in adult macaques by experimental frontal lobe lesions. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 13, 211–224.
Rosvold, H. E., & Mishkin, M. (1950). Evaluation of the effects of prefrontal lobotomy on intelligence. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 4, 122–126.
Rushworth, G., & Denny-Brown, D. (1959). The two components of the grasp reflex after ablation of frontal cortex in monkeys. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 51, 91–98.
Russell, W. R. (1959). Brain, memory and learning. Fair Lawn, NJ: Oxford University Press.
Rylander, G. (1939). Personality changes after operations on the frontal lobes: A clinical study of thirty-two cases. Acta Psychiat. Neurol., Copenhagen, Suppl. 20.
Rylander, G. (1943). Mental changes after excision of cerebral tissue. A clinical study of 16 cases of resections in the parietal, temporal and occipital lobes. Acta Psychiat. Neurol., Coppenhagen, Suppl. 25, 80.
Schuster, P., & Pinéas, H. (1926). Weitere Beobachtungen über Zwangsgreifen und Nachgreifen. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde, 91, 16–56.
Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1954). Performance on complex tactual tasks after brain injury in man: Analyses by locus of lesion. American Journal of Psychology, 67, 220–240.
Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1955). Spatial orientation in man after cerebral injury: I. Analyses by locus of lesion. Journal of Psychology, 39, 227–244.
Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1960). Somato-sensory changes after penetrating brain wounds in man. Commonwealth Fund. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1963). Impaired orientation in personal and extrapersonal space. Brain, 86 (in press).
Silberpfennig, J. (1941). Contributions to the problem of eye movements: III. Disturbances of ocular movements with pseudohemianopsia in frontal lobe tumors. Confinia Neurologica, 4, 1–13.
Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 43, 482–489.
Teuber, H.-L. (1955). Physiological psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 6, 267–296.
Teuber, H.-L. (1959). Some alterations in behavior after cerebral lesions in man. In A. D. Bass (Ed.), Evolution of nervous control from primitive organisms in man (pp. 157–194). Washington, DC: Amer. Ass. Adv. Sci.
Teuber, H.-L. (1960). Perception. Chap. LXV. In J. Field, H. W. Magcun, & V. E. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of Physiology, Section 1. Neurophysiology (Vol. 3). Washington, DC: Am. Physiol. Soc.
Teuber, H.-L. (1961a). Sensory deprivation, sensory suppression and agnosia: Notes for a neurologic theory. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 132, 32–40.
Teuber, H.-L. (1961b). Brain and behavior (summation). In M. A. B. Brazier (Ed.), Int. Conf. on Brain and Behavior (pp. 393–420). Washington, DC: Amer. Inst. Biol. Sci.
Teuber, H.-L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1949). Changes in visual searching performance following cerebral lesions. American Journal of Physiology, 159, 592.
Teuber, H.-L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1951). Performance of complex visual tasks after cerebral lesions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 114, 413–429.
Teuber, H.-L., Battersby, W. S., & Bender, M. B. (1960). Visual field defects after penetrating missile wounds of the brain. Commonwealth Fund. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Teuber, H.-L., & Bender, M. B. (1948). Critical flicker frequency in defective fields of vision. Fed. Proc. Amer. Soc. Exp. Biol., 7, 123–124.
Teuber, H.-L., & Bender, M. B. (1949). Alterations in pattern vision following trauma of occipital lobes in man. Journal of General Psychology, 40, 37–57.
Teuber, H.-L., & Liebert, R. S. (1958). Specific and general effects of brain injury in man. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, Chicago, 80, 403–407.
Teuber, H.-L., & Mishkin, M. (1954). Judgment of visual and postural vertical after brain injury. Journal of Psychology, 38, 161–175.
Teuber, H.-L., & Rudel, R. G. (1962). Behaviour after cerebral lesions in children and adults. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 4, 3–20.
Teuber, H.-L., & Weinstein, S. (1954). Performance on a formboard-task after penetrating brain injury. Journal of Psychology, 38, 177–190.
Teuber, H.-L., & Weinstein, S. (1956). Ability to discover hidden figures after cerebral lesions. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, Chicago, 76, 369–379.
Teuber, H.-L., & Weinstein, S. (1958). Equipotentiality versus cortical localization. Science, 127, 241–242.
Towe, A., & Jabbur, S. J. (1961). Cortical inhibition of neurons in dorsal column nuclei of cat. Journal of Neurophysiology, 24, 488–489.
von Monakow, C. (1910). Der rote Kern, die Haube und die Regio hypothalamica bei einigen Säugetieren und beim Menschen. Arbeiten aus dem Hirnanatomischen Institut in Zürich, 4, 103–226.
Walberg, F. (1957). Corticofugal fibres to the nuclei of the dorsal columns. An experimental study in the cat. Brain, 80, 273–287.
Weigl, E. (1941). On the psychology of so-called processes of abstraction. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36, 3–33 (transl. by Margaret J. Rioch from the original: “Zur Psychologie sogenannter Abstraktionsprozesse,” Z. Psychol., 1927, 103, 2–45).
Weinstein, S., Semmes, J., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H.-L. (1956). Spatial orientation in man after cerebral injury; II. Analyses according to concomitant defects. Journal of Psychology, 42, 249–263.
Weinstein, S., & Teuber, H.-L. (1957). Effects of penetrating brain injury on intelligence test scores. Science, 125, 1036–1037.
Weinstein, S., Teuber, H.-L., Ghent, L., & Semmes, J. (1955). Complex visual task performance after penetrating brain injury in man. American Psychologist, 10, 408.
Welch, K., & Stuteville, P. (1958). Experimental production of unilateral neglect in monkeys. Brain, 81, 341–347.
Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex (p. 72). Max Cohn and Weigert: Breslau.
Wernicke, C. (1894–1896). Grundiss der Psychiatrie in klinischen Vorlesungen. Leipzig: Thieme.
Yacorzynski, G. K., Boshes, B., & Davis, L. (1948). Psychological changes produced by frontal lobotomy. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 27, 642–657.
Zangwill, O. L. (1951). Discussion on parietal lobe syndromes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine (Sect. Neurol.), 44, 343–346.
Additional information
This article has been re-typeset for clarity but the content is unchanged from the original version published as Teuber, HL. The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In JM Warren and K Akert. (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw Hill, pp. 410–477 (1964). Copyright 1964 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.
1The program of research on which this study is based is carried out at the Psychophysiological Laboratory of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. The laboratory receives support from the Rockefeller and Hanford Foundations, and from the National Institutes of Health (under program grant M-S673). Additional aid is being received from the Life Sciences and Behavioral Sciences Programs of the Office of Scientific Research, U.S. Air Force, and from the Behavioral Biology Program of the National Aeronautics and Space Administration, under NASA grant NSG-496. In its earlier phases while at the Bellevue Medical Center in New York, from 1948 to 1962, the laboratory has been supported by the Commonwealth Fund of New York, by the U.S. Veterans Administration, and by the Office of the Surgeon General, U.S. Army, under Contract DA-49-007-MD-312.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Teuber, HL. The Riddle of Frontal Lobe Function in Man. Neuropsychol Rev 19, 25–46 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9088-z
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9088-z