Neuropsychology Review

, Volume 17, Issue 3, pp 213–233 | Cite as

The Elusive Nature of Executive Functions: A Review of our Current Understanding

Article

Abstract

Executive functions include abilities of goal formation, planning, carrying out goal-directed plans, and effective performance. This article aims at reviewing some of the current knowledge surrounding executive functioning and presenting the contrasting views regarding this concept. The neural substrates of the executive system are examined as well as the evolution of executive functioning, from development to decline. There is clear evidence of the vulnerability of executive functions to the effects of age over lifespan. The first executive function to emerge in children is the ability to inhibit overlearned behavior and the last to appear is verbal fluency. Inhibition of irrelevant information seems to decline earlier than set shifting and verbal fluency during senescence. The sequential progression and decline of these functions has been paralleled with the anatomical changes of the frontal lobe and its connections with other brain areas. Generalization of the results presented here are limited due to methodological differences across studies. Analysis of these differences is presented and suggestions for future research are offered.

Keywords

Executive Functions Frontal lobe Elderly Children Development 

References

  1. Acevedo, A., Lowenstein, D. A., Barker, W. W., Harwood, D. G., Luis, C., & Bravo, M., et al. (2000). Category fluency test: A normative data for English and Spanish-speaking elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 760–769.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, V. (2001). Assessing executive functions in children: Biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 4, 119–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson, P., Anderson, V., & Lajoie, G. (1996). The Tower of London Test: Validation and standardization for pediatric population. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10(1), 64–65.Google Scholar
  5. Anderson, V., Anderson, P., Northam, E., Jacobs, R., & Catroppa, C. (2001a). Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 385–406.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Anderson, V., Fenwick, T., Manly, T., & Robertson, I. (1998). Attentional skills following traumatic brain injury in childhood: A componential analysis. Brain Injury, 12, 937–949.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Anderson, V., Levin, H., & Jacobs, R. (2002). Executive functions after frontal lobe injury: A developmental perspective. In D. T. Stuss, & R. T. Knight (Eds.) Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 504–527). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Anderson, V., Northam, E., Hendy, J., & Wrenall, J. (2001b). Developmental neuropsychology: A clinical approach. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  9. Andrés, P., & van der Linden, M. (2000). Age-related differences in supervisory attentional system functions. Journal of Gerontology, 55, 373–380.Google Scholar
  10. Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solis, F., Rosselli, M., & Gomez, C. (2000a). Age-related cognitive decline: The complex effect of education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 495–513.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Ardila, A., Pineda, D., & Rosselli, M. (2000b). Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 31–36.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, G. (2005). The influence of the parents’ educational level on the development of executive functions. Developmental Neuropyschology, 28, 539–560.Google Scholar
  13. Ardila, A., & Surloff, C. (2002). Dysexecutive syndrome. www.Medlink.com, Neurology.
  14. Ardila, A., & Surloff, C. (2004). Dysexecutive syndromes. Medlink Neurology. San Diego: Arbor Publishing Co.Google Scholar
  15. Army Individual Test Battery (1944). Manual of directions and scoring. Washington, DC: War Department, Adjutant General’s Office.Google Scholar
  16. Audenaert, K., Brans, B., van Laere, K., Lahorte, P., Versijpt, J., & van Heeringen, K., et al. (2000). Verbal fluency as a prefrontal activation probe: A validation study using 99mTc-ECD brain SPET. European Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 27, 1800–1808.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Auriacombe, S., Fabriogoule, C., Lafont, S., Amieva, H., Jacquim-Gadda, H., & Dartigues, J. F. (2001). Letter and category fluency in normal elderly participants: A population based study. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 8, 98–108.Google Scholar
  18. Axelrod, B. N., & Henry R. R. (1992). Age-related performance on the Wisconsin card sorting, similarities, and controlled oral word association tests. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(1), 16–26.Google Scholar
  19. Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 225, 556–559.Google Scholar
  21. Baddeley, A. (1998). The central executive: A concept and some misconceptions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 523–526.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Baddeley, A. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. In D. T. Stuss, & R. T. Knight (Eds.) Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 246–260). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.) Recent advances in learning and motivation (vol. 8). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  24. Baddeley, A., & Logie, R. (1999). Working memory: The multiple-component model. In A. Miyake, & P. Shah (Eds.) Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 28–61). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. (1988). Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 7, 212–230.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Band, G., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Segalowitz, S. (2002). Explaining neurocognitive aging: Is one factor enough. Brain and Cognition, 49, 259–267.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Banich, M. T. (2004). Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  28. Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Belleville, S., Rouleau, N., & van der Linden, M. (2006). Use of the Hayling task to measure inhibition of prepotent responses in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 62, 113–119.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. (1989). Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA.Google Scholar
  31. Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 15–22.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Berman, K., Ostrem, J., Randolph, C., Gold, J., Goldberg, T., Coppola, R., Carson, R., Herscovitch, P., & Weinberger, D. (1995). Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1027–1046.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Bialystok, E., Craik, F., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Boone, K. B. (1999). Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions: Impact of age, education, gender, intellectual level, and vascular status on executive test scores. In B. L. Miller, & J. L. Cummings (Eds.) The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders (pp. 247–261). New York: Guildford.Google Scholar
  35. Borkowsky, J. G., & Burke, J. E. (1996). Theories, models and measurements of executive functioning: An information processing perspective. In G. R. Lyon, & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.) Attention, memory and executive function. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  36. Brennan, M., Welsh, M. C., & Fisher, C. B. (1997). Aging and executive function skills: An examination of a community-dwelling older population. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 1187–1197.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Brickman, A., Paul, R., Cohen, R., William, L., MacGreggor, K., Jefferson, A., Tate, D., Gunstad, J., & Gordon, E. (2005). Category and letter verbal fluency across the adult lifespan: Relationship to EEG theta power. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 561–573.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(2), 571–593.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Bryan, J., & Luszcz, M. (2001). Adult age differences in self-ordered pointing task performance: Contributions from working memory, executive function and speed of information processing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 608–619.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Burgess, P. (1997). Theory and methodology in executive function research. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.) Methodology of frontal executive function (pp. 81–116). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  41. Burgess, P., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 547–558.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Brixton Tests. Bury St. Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test.Google Scholar
  43. Cahn-Weiner, D., Malloy, P., Boyle, P., Marran, M., & Salloway, S. (2000). Prediction of functional status from neuropsychological tests in community-dwelling elderly individuals. Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 187–195.Google Scholar
  44. Catafau, A., Parellada, E., Lomena, F., Bernardo, M., Setoain, J., & Catarineu, S., et al. (1998). Role of the cingulate gyrus during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A single photon emission computed tomography study in normal volunteers. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 26, 67–74.Google Scholar
  45. Chaytor, N., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., & Burr, R. (2006). Improving the ecological validity of executive functioning assessment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 217–227.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Annals of Neurology, 22, 287–297.Google Scholar
  47. Collette, T., & van der Linden, M. (2002). Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavior Review, 26, 105–125.Google Scholar
  48. Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., & Salmon, E. (2001). The functional anatomy of inhibition processes investigated with the Hayling task. Neuroimage, 14, 258–267.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Collette, T., van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Delfiore, G., Degueldre, C., & Luxen, A., et al. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 409–423.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Crawford, J., Bryan, J., Luszcz, M., Obonsawin, M., & Stewart, L. (2000). The executive decline hypothesis of cognitive aging: Do executive deficits qualify as differential deficit and do they mediate age-related memory decline? Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 7, 9–31.Google Scholar
  51. Crawford, S., & Channon, S. (2002). Dissociation between performance on abstract tests of executive function and problem solving in real-life type situations in normal aging. Aging & Mental Health, 6, 12–21.Google Scholar
  52. Crossley, M., D’Arcy, C., & Rawson, N. S. (1997). Letter and category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 52–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Crinella, F., & Yu, J. (2000). Brain mechanisms and intelligence psychometric g and executive functions. Intelligence, 27(4), 299–327.Google Scholar
  54. Dagher, A., Owen, A., Boecker, H., & Brooks, D. (1999). Mapping the network for planning: A correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. Brain, 122, 1973–1987.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Dahua, W., Jiliang, S., Huamao, P., Dan, T., & Liang, Z. (2005). The model of educational effect on old adults’ cognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 37, 511–516.Google Scholar
  56. Daigneault, S., & Braun, C. M. (1993). Working memory and the self-ordered pointing task: Further evidence of early prefrontal decline in normal aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 881–895.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Daigneault, S., Braun, C. M., & Whitaker, H. A. (1992). Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non-perseverative prefrontal measures. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 99–114.Google Scholar
  58. Daigneault, G., Joly, P., & Frigon, J. Y. (2002). Executive functions in the evaluation of accident risk of older individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2, 221–238.Google Scholar
  59. Davis, H., & Klebe, K. (2001). A longitudinal study of the performance of the elderly and young on the Tower of Hanoi puzzle and Rey recall. Brain and Cognition, 46, 95–99.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. De Frias, C., Dixon, R., & Strauss, E. (2006). Structure of tour executive functioning tests in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology, 20, 206–214.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Delis, D., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, N. (2001). Delis–Kaplan executive function system. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  62. De Luca, C., Wood, S., Anderson, V., Buchanan, J., Proffit, R., & Mahony, K., et al. (2003). Normative data from the Cantab: Development of executive function over the lifespan. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 242–254.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Denckla, M. B. (1996). A theory and model of executive function: A neuropsychological perspective. In G. Lyon, & N. Krasnegor (Eds.) Attention, memory and executive function. Maryland: Paul Brooks.Google Scholar
  64. Desman, C., Schneider, A., Ziegler-Kirbach, E., Peterman, F., Mohr, B., & Hampel, P. (2006). Behavioral inhibition and emotion regulation among boys with ADHD during a go-no-go task. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 55, 328–349.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. D’Esposito, M., Postle, B., Ballard, D., & Lease, J. (1999). Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: An event-related fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 41, 66–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood. In D. T. Stuss, & R. T. Knight (Eds.) Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466–503). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. (1985). Evidence for involvement of prefrontal cortex in cognitive changes during the first year of life: Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on a detour task with transparent barrier. Neurosciences Abstracts, 11, 832.Google Scholar
  68. Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. (1989). Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget’s AB task: Evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 74, 24–40.Google Scholar
  69. Dickstein, S., Bannon, K., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. (2006). The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: An ALE meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1051–1062.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Du Boisegueheneuc, F., Levy, R., Volle, E., Seassau, M., Duffau, H., & Kinkingneheun, S., et al. (2006). Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: A lesion study. Brain, 129, 3315–3328.Google Scholar
  71. Duncan, J., Burgess, P., & Emslie, H. (1995). Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsichologia, 33, 261–268.Google Scholar
  72. Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., & Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobes: The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257–303.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Eldreth, D., Patterson, M., Porcelli, A., Biswal, B., Rebbechi, D., & Rypma, B. (2006). Evidence for multiple manipulation processes in prefrontal cortex. Brain Research, 1123, 145–156.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Elliot, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 65, 49–59.Google Scholar
  75. Espy, K. (1997). The shape school: Assessing executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 495–499.Google Scholar
  76. Fassbender, C., Murphy, K., Foxe, J., Wylie, G., Javitt, D., & Robertson, I., et al. (2004). A topography of executive functions and their interactions revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Research, Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 132–143, happiness.Google Scholar
  77. Fisk, J. E., & Sharp, C. A. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: Updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 874–890.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Fisk, J. E., & Warr, P. (1996). Age and working memory: The role of perceptual speed, the central executive and the phonological loop. Psychology and Aging, 11, 316–323.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Defries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172–179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Frith, C., Friston, K., Liddle, P., & Frackowiak, R. (1991). A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia, 29, 1137–1148.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Fuster, J. (1993). Frontal lobes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3, 160–165.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Fuster, J. (2002). Physiology of executive functions: The perception–action cycle. In D. T. Stuss, & R. T. Knight (Eds.) Principles of the frontal lobe. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  83. Garden, S., Phillips, L., & MacPherson, S. (2001). Midlife aging, open-ended planning, and laboratory measures of executive function. Neuropsychology, 15, 472–482.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Gerstadt, C., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3 1/2–7 years old on a Stroop-like day–night test. Cognition, 53, 129–153.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Gerton, B., Brown, T., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Kohn, P., Holt, J., & Olsen, R., et al. (2004). Shared and distinct neurophysiological components of the digits forward and backward tasks as revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1781–1787.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Gioia, G., & Isquith, P. (2004). Ecological assessment of executive function in traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 135–158.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive functions. Lutz, FL: Psycological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  88. Girgis, R., Minshew, N., Melhem, N., Nutche, J., Keshavan, M. S., & Hardan, A. (2007). Volumetric alterations of the orbitofrontal cortex in autism. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology, & Biological Psychiatry, 31, 41–45.Google Scholar
  89. Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disorders of executive function among aphasic and other brain-damaged patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 485–501.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Godefroy, O., Cabaret, M., Petit-Chenal, V., Pruvo, J.-P., & Rousseaux, M. (1999). Control functions of the frontal lobe: Modularity of the central-supervisory system. Cortex, 35, 1–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Goethals, I., Audenaert, K., Jacobs, F., van der Wiele, C., Pyck, H., & Ham, H., et al. (2004). Application of a neuropsychological activation probe with SPECT: The ‘Tower of London’ task in healthy volunteers. Nuclear Medicine Communications, 25, 177–182.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Golberg, E. (2001). The executive brain: Frontal lobes and the civilized mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  93. Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop color and word test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.Google Scholar
  94. Grace, J., & Malloy, P. F. (2002). Frontal system behavioral scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  95. Grafman, J. (1999). Experimental assessment of adult frontal lobe. In B. L. Miller, & J. L. Cummings (Eds.) The human frontal lobes (pp. 321–344). New York: Guildford.Google Scholar
  96. Greenwood, P. (2000). The frontal aging hypothesis evaluated. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 705–726.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Grigsby, J., Kaye, K., Shetterly, S. M., Baxter, J., Morgenstern, N., & Hamman, R. F. (2002). Prevalence of disorders of executive cognitive functioning among the elderly: Findings from the San Luis Valley health and aging study. Neuroepidemiology, 21, 213–220.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Grigsby, J., Kaye, K., Baxter, J., Shetterly, S. M., & Hamman, R. F. (1998). Executive cognitive abilities and functional status among community-dwelling older persons in the San Luis Valley Health and Aging Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 46, 590–596.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Haaland, K., Vranes, L., Goodwin, J., & Garry, P. (1987). Wisconsin Card Sort performance in a healthy elderly population. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 345–346.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Haarmann, H., Ashling, G., Davelaar, E., & Usher, M. (2005). Age-related declines in context maintenance and semantic short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 58, 34–53.Google Scholar
  101. Harris, P. L. (1974). Perseverative search at a visibly empty place by young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 535–542.Google Scholar
  102. Hasher, L., Tonev, S. T., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. T. (2001). Inhibitory control, environmental support, and self-initiated processing in aging. In M. Naveh-Benjamin, M. Moscovitch, & R. L. Roediger (Eds.) Perspectives on human memory and cognitive aging: Essays in honour of Fergus Craik. East Sussex: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  103. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  104. Hashimoto, R., Meguro, K., Lee, E., Kasai, M., Ishii, H., & Yamaguchi, S. (2006). Effect of age and education on the TMT and determination of normative data for Japanese elderly people: The Tajin Project. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 60, 422–428.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Haug, H., & Eggers, R. (1991). Morphometry of the human cortex cerebri and corpus striatum during aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 12, 336–338.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Hill, E. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 26–32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Hirshorn, E. A., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2006). Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: Neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2547–2557.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Hobson, P., & Leeds, L. (2001). Executive functioning in older people. Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, 11, 361–372.Google Scholar
  109. Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., & Sosa, B. B. (1995). Planning in the real world: Preschool children’s scripts and plans for familiar events. Child Development, 66, 984–998.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131–142.Google Scholar
  111. Hughes, C., Russel, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Huppert, F., Bravne, F., Paykel, E., & Beardsall, L. (1995). CAMCOG—a concise neuropsychological test to assist dementia diagnosis: Socio-demographic determinants in an elderly population sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 529–541.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Hurks, P., Vles, J., Hendriksen, J., Kalff, A., Feron, F., & Kroes, M., et al. (2006). Semantic category fluency versus initial letter fluency over 60 seconds as a measure of automatic and controlled processing in healthy school-aged children. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 684–695.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  114. Isquith, P., Gioia, G., & Espy, K. (2004). Executive function in preschool children: Examination through everyday behavior. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 403–422.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Izaks, G., & Westendorp, R. (2003). Ill or just old? Towards a conceptual framework on the relation between aging and disease. BMC Geriatrics, 3, 7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R., & Frith, C. (2000). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: A study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage, 12, 713–725.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Kassubek, J., Juengling, F. D., Ecker, D., & Landwehrmeyer, G. B. (2005). Thalamic atrophy in Huntington’s disease co-varies with cognitive performance: A morphometric MRI analysis. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 846–853.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Kaufmann, L., Koppelstaetter, F., Delazer, M., Siedentopf, C., Rhomberg, P., & Golaszewski, S., et al. (2005). Neural correlates of distance and congruity effects in a numerical Stroop task: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 15, 888–898.Google Scholar
  119. Keys, B., & White, D. (2000). Exploring the relationship between age, executive abilities, and psychomotor speed. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 76–82.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Kimberg, D., D’Esposito, M., & Farah, M. (1997). Cognitive functions in the prefrontal cortex—working memory and executive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 185–192.Google Scholar
  121. Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3 to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 407–428.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Koechlin, E., Corrado, G., Pietrini, P., & Grafman, J. (2000). Dissociating. the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 97, 7651–7656.Google Scholar
  123. Lafleche, G., & Albert, M. (1995). Executive function deficits in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 9, 313–320.Google Scholar
  124. Lazeron, R. H., Rombouts, S. A., Machielsen, W. C., Scheltens, P., Witter, M. P., & Uylings, H. B., et al. (2000). Visualizing brain activation during planning: The tower of London test adapted for functional MR imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 21, 1407–1414.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  125. Lehto, J. (1996). Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 29–50.Google Scholar
  126. Levin, H., Culhane, K., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., Mattson, A., & Harwood, H. (1991). Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 377–396.Google Scholar
  127. Lewis, S. J., Dove, A., Robbins, T. W., Barker, R. A., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Striatal contributions to working memory: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 755–760.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  128. Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  129. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  130. Lie, C., Specht, K., Marshall, J., & Fink, G. (2006). Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage, 15, 1038–1049.Google Scholar
  131. Lombardi, W., Andreason, P., Sirocco, K., Rio, D., Gross, R., & Umhau, J., et al. (1999). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance following head injury: Dorsolateral fronto-striatal circuit activity predicts perseveration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 2–16.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Luria, A. R. (1973). The Working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
  133. Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Tonev, S. (2001). Inhibitory control over the present and the past. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 107–122.Google Scholar
  134. MacPherson, S., Phillips, L., & DellaSala, S. (2002). Age, executive function, and social decision making: A dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 4, 598–609.Google Scholar
  135. Mani, T., Bedwell, J., & Miller, S. (2005). Age-related decrements in performance on a brief continuous performance test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 575–586.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  136. Manly, J., Schumpf, N., Tang, M., & Stern, Y. (2005). Cognitive decline and literacy among ethnically diverse elders. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 18, 213–217.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  137. Matute, E., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., & Morales, L. (2004). Verbal and non-verbal fluency in Spanish speaking children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(2), 647–660.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  138. Mejia, S., Pineda, D., Alvarez, L., & Ardila, A. (1998). Individual Differences in memory and executive function abilities during normal aging. International Journal of Neuroscience, 95, 271–284.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Miller, A. K., Alston, R. L., & Corselli, J. A. (1980). Variation with age in the volumes of grey and white matter in the cerebral hemispheres of man: Measurements with an image analyzer. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, 6, 119–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Mitrushina, K. M., Boone, K. B., Razani, J., & D’Elia, L. F. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment, second edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  141. Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  142. Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Strafella, A. P., Worsley, K. J., & Doyon, J. (2006). Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions. Annuals of Neurology, 59, 257–264.Google Scholar
  143. Morris, R., Ahmed, S., Syed, G., & Toone, B. (1993). Neural correlates of planning ability: Frontal lobe activation during the Tower of London test. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1367–1378.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  144. Nagahama, Y., Okada, T., Katsumi, Y., Hayashi, Y., Yamauchi, H., & Oyanagi, Y., et al. (2001). Dissociable mechanisms of attentional control within the human prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 85–92.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  145. Nathan, J., Wilkinson, D., Stammers, S., & Low, L. (2001). The role of tests of frontal executive function in the detection of mild dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 18–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  146. Nelson, H. E. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex, 12(4), 313–324.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  147. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, et al. (Ed.) Consciousness and self-regulation (vol. 4, (pp. 1–18)). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  148. Norris, G., & Tate, R. (2000). The Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): Ecological, concurrent and construct validity. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 11, 33–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Obonsawin, M. C., Crawford, J. R., Page, J., Chalmers, P., Cochrane, R., & Low, G. (2002). Performance on test of frontal lobe function reflect general intellectual ability. Neuropsychologia, 40, 970–977.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  150. Odhuba, R., van den Broek, M., & Johns, L. (2005). Ecological validity of measures of executive functioning. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 269–278.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  151. Ostrosky-Solis, F., Gutierrez, A. L., Flores, M. R., & Ardila, A. (2007). Same or different? Semantic verbal fluency across Spanish-speakers from different countries. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 367–377.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  152. Owen, A., Doyon, J., Petrides, M., & Evans, A. (1996). Planning and spatial working memory: A positron emission tomography study in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 353–364.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1998). Inhibitory deficits in Tourette’s syndrome: A function of comorbidity and symptom severity. Journal of Child Psychiatry, 39, 1109–1118.Google Scholar
  154. Parkin, A. (1998). The central executive does not exist. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 518–522.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  155. Parkin, A., & Java, R. (1999). Deterioration of frontal lobe function in normal aging: Influences of fluid intelligence versus perceptual speed. Neuropsychology, 13, 539–545.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  156. Parkin, A., Walter, B., & Hunkin, N. (1995). Relationship between normal aging, frontal lobe function, and memory for temporal and spatial information. Neuropsychology, 9, 304–312.Google Scholar
  157. Passler, M., Issac, W., & Hynd, G. (1985). Neuropsychological development of behavior attributed to the frontal lobe functioning in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 1(4), 349–370.Google Scholar
  158. Paulesu, E., Goldacre, B., Scifo, P., Cappa, S., Gilardi, M., & Castiglioni, I., et al. (1997). Functional heterogeneity of left inferior frontal cortex as revealed by fMRI. Neuroreport, 27, 2011–2017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O., & Roberts, R. J. (1996). Executive functions and working memory; theoretical and measurement issues. In G. R. Lyon, & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.) Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
  160. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  161. Perianez, J. A., Maestu, F., Barcelo, F., Fernandez, A., Amo, C., & Ortiz-Alonso, T. (2004). Spatiotemporal brain dynamics during preparatory set shifting: MEG evidence. Neuroimage, 21, 687–695.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Phelps, E., Hyder, F., Blamire, A., & Shulman, R. (1997). FMRI of the prefrontal cortex during overt verbal fluency. Neuroreport, 8, 561–565.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  163. Phillips, L. H., Kliegel, M., & Martin, M. (2006). Age and planning tasks: The influence of ecological validity. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 62, 175–184.Google Scholar
  164. Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. Oxford: Basic.Google Scholar
  165. Piguet, O., Grayson, G., Browe, A., Tate, H., Lye, T., & Creasey, H., et al. (2002). Normal aging and executive functions in “old-old” community dwellers: Poor performance is not an inevitable outcome. International Psychogeriatric Association, 14, 139–159.Google Scholar
  166. Pihlajamaki, M., Tanila, H., Hanninen, T., Kononen, M., Laakso, M., & Partanen, K., et al. (2000). Verbal fluency activates the left medial temporal lobe: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Annals of Neurology, 47, 1367–1313.Google Scholar
  167. Plumet, J., Gil, R., & Gaonac’h, D. (2005). Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions in women: Effects of age and education. Neuropsychology, 19(5), 566–577.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  168. Racine, C. A., Barch, D. M., Braver, T. S., & Noelle, D. C. (2006). The effect of age on rule-based category learning. Neuropsychological Development and Cognition: Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 13, 411–434.Google Scholar
  169. Rapp, M., & Reischies, F. (2005). Attention and executive control predict Alzheimer’s disease in late life: Results from the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 134–141.Google Scholar
  170. Reitan, R. M. (1955). The relation of the Trail Making Test to organic brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 19, 393–394.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  171. Rekkas, V. (2006). Interference resolution in the elderly: Evidence suggestive of differences in strategy on measures of prepotent inhibition and dual task processing. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 13, 341–365.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  172. Ridderinkhof, K. R., Span, M. M., & van der Molen, M. W. (2002). Perseverative behavior and adaptive control in older adults: Performance monitoring, rule induction, and set shifting. Brain and Cognition, 49, 382–401.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  173. Riva, D., Nichelli, F., & Devoti, M. (2000). Developmental aspects of verbal fluency and confrontation naming in children. Brain and Language, 71, 267–284.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  174. Robbins, T., James, M., Owen, A., Sahakian, B., Lawrence, A., & Mcinnes, L., et al. (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB batter sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 474–490.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  175. Rodriguez-Aranda, C., & Martinussen, M. (2006). Age-related decline in performance of phonetic verbal fluency measured by the Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT): A meta-analysis study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30, 697–717.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  176. Romine, C., & Reynolds, C. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190–201.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  177. Rönnlund, M., Lövden, M., & Nilsson, L. G. (2001). Adult age differences in Tower of Hanoi performance: Influence from demographic and cognitive variables. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 8, 269–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  178. Royall, P., Lauterbach, E. C., Cummings, J. L., Reeve, A., Rummans, T. A., & Kaufer, D. I., et al. (2002). Executive control function: A review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 377–405.Google Scholar
  179. Rush, B. K., Barch, D. M., & Braver, T. S. (2006). Accounting for cognitive aging: Context processing, inhibition or processing speed? Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition. Section B: Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 13, 588–610.Google Scholar
  180. Salat, D. H., Tuch, D. S., Henelone, N. D., Fischl, B., Corkin, S., & Rosas, H. D., et al. (2005). Age related changes in pre-frontal white matter measure by diffusion tensor imaging. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1064, 37–49.Google Scholar
  181. Salthouse, T. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  182. Salthouse, T. (2005). Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology, 19, 532–545.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  183. Salthouse, T., Atkinson, T., & Berish, D. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594.Google Scholar
  184. Salthouse, T. A., Siedlecki, K. L., & Krueger, L. E. (2006). An individual differences analysis of memory control. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 102–125.Google Scholar
  185. Salthouse, T., Toth, J., Daniels, K., Parks, C., Pak, R., & Wolbrette, M., et al. (2000). Effects of aging on efficiency of task switching in a variant of the trail making test. Neuropsychology, 14, 102–111.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  186. Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J., Geurts, H., Morein-Zamir, S., Meiran, N., & Schut, H., et al. (2004). Executive functioning in boys with ADHD: Primarily an inhibition deficit? Archives in Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 569–594.Google Scholar
  187. Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, S., Murphy, D. (2006). Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 54(1), 7–16.Google Scholar
  188. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 298, 199–209.Google Scholar
  189. Shallice, T. (2002). Fractionation of the supervisory system. In D. T. Stuss, & T. Knight (Eds.) Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 261–277). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  190. Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Higher-order cognitive impairments and frontal lobe lesions in man. In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton (Eds.) Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 125–138). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  191. Siegel, B., Nuechterlein, K., Abel, L., Wu, J., & Buchsbaum, M. (1995). Glucose metabolic correlates of continuous performance test performance in adults with a history of infantile autism, schizophrenics, and controls. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 85–94.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  192. Simon, H. A. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 268–288.Google Scholar
  193. Slattery, M., Garvey, M., & Swedo, S. (2001). Frontal–subcortical circuits: A functional developmental approach. In D. G. Lichter, & J. L. Cummings (Eds.) Frontal–subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 314–333). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  194. Span, M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & van der Molen, W. (2004). Age-related changes in the efficiency of cognitive processing across the life span. Acta Psychologica, 117, 155–183.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  195. Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  196. Springer, M., McIntosh, A., Winocur, G., & Grady, C. (2005). The relation between brain activity during memory tasks and years of education in young and older adults. Neuropsychology, 19, 181–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  197. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  198. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.Google Scholar
  199. Stuss, D. T. (2000). New approaches to prefrontal lobe testing. In B. Miller, & I. Cummings (Eds.) The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders. London: Giuldford.Google Scholar
  200. Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63, 289–298.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  201. Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Floden, D., Binns, M. A., Levine, B., & McIntosh, A. R., et al. (2002). Fractionation and localization of distinct frontal lobe processes: Evidence from focal lesions in humans. In D. T. Stuss, & R. T. Knight (Eds.) Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 392–407). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  202. Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal lobes. New York: Raven.Google Scholar
  203. Van der Elst, W., van Boxtel, M., van Breukelen, G., & Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop color–word test: Influence of age, sex and education; normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment, 13, 62–79.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  204. Wagner, G., Kock, K., Reichenbach, J., Sauer, H., & Schlosser, R. (2006). The special involvement of the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in planning abilities: An event-related fMRI study with the Tower of London paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2337–2347.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  205. Wager, T. D., & Smith, E. E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 241–253.Google Scholar
  206. Wecker, N. S., Kramer, J. H., Hallam, B. J., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Mental flexibility: Age effects on switching. Neuropsychology, 19, 345–352.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  207. Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groissier, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of executive functions: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  208. Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., Ozonoff, S., Rouse, B., & McCAbe, E. R. (1990). Neuropsychology of early-treated phenylketonuria: Specific executive function deficits. Child Development, 61, 1697–1713.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  209. West, R. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  210. Williams, B., Ponesse, J., Schacher, R., Logan, G., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 35, 205–213.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  211. Wood, R., & Liossi, C. (2006). The ecological validity of executive tests in a severely brain injured sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 429–437.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  212. Yoon, D., Gause, C., Leckman, F., & Singer, H. (2007). Frontal dopaminergic abnormality in Tourette syndrome: A postmortem analysis. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 255, 50–56.Google Scholar
  213. Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J., & Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive functions: A problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 198–226.Google Scholar
  214. Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F., & Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the lifespan. Acta Psychologica, 115, 167–183.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  215. Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D. (1998). II. Cognitive complexity and control: The development of executive function. Current directions in Psychological Science, 7, 121–126.Google Scholar
  216. Zelazo, P. D., & Muller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.) Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  217. Zook, N., Welsh, M., & Ewing, V. (2006). Performance of healthy, older adults on the Tower of London Revised: Associations with verbal and nonverbal abilities. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 1–19.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Charles Schmidt College of ScienceFlorida Atlantic UniversityDavieUSA

Personalised recommendations