Skip to main content
Log in

Two or III Feet Apart: Oral Recitation, Roman Numerals, and Metrical Regularity in Capystranus

  • Published:
Neophilologus Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Middle English verse romance, Capystranus, contains 32 numerical expressions in fewer than 580 lines, some of which are spelled out, others are in Roman numerals, while the rest are in mixed orthographical/numerical forms. Values range between one and 100,000 and contain up to four individual lexemes requiring as many as six syllables to be expressed orally. Although it was initially suspected that such complex expression might be silently “skipped over”, the verse shows that the poem’s metrical regularity is dependent upon full vocal/acoustic realization of all numbers expressed in Roman notation. While this does not preclude silent reading, it does at least suggest the text was suitable for oral recitation. This paper shows how even complex Roman numerals were either suitable or suitably adapted for quick and easy interpretation. Propositions are provided concerning the choice of orthographical representation or Roman notation and its treatment. In this romance it seems that line length was less important than a number’s position in a line, as well as the ease with which a number could be “subitized” or mentally visualized and approximated, resulting in high frequency, low-value numbers usually being realized as lexical items to be written out, while high-value numbers conceived of more abstractly would be represented symbolically. The exceptions seem to be certain high frequency numbers of account (e.g., 20, 100, 1000), which could be provided orthographically or numerically despite difficulty subitizing them, while common low-value numbers in time expressions could be expressed numerically as they would appear on time pieces.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Ropa here references Edwards on Morte Darthur (1996, p. 243).

  2. Shepherd’s edition (based on London, BL, incunable C. 71. c. 26) provides 579 lines. The online edition provided by the University of Otago, however, convincingly purports 3 missing lines due to lacunae in the rhyme schemes (between Shepherd’s lines 81–82, lines 114–115, and lines 162–163), and also suggests 13 more missing lines due to conflation of two stanzas at Shepherd’s lines 354–355. Although line numbers cited here are drawn from Otago, which is preferred due to its relative accessibility, only the 579 lines listed by Shepherd actually appear in the extant incunabula.

  3. Technically a logograph might give some indication of pronunciation, whereas ideographs are purely indicative of a concept. See footnote 6 (below) for more information.

  4. Picard notes that this was largely due to the fact that the Arabic numerals facilitated the use of double-entry book-keeping, which city merchants in London had imported from Italy, and which was increasingly common in city counting houses (2017, pp. 50, 71).

  5. Entry for 29 May in an e-book with no pagination.

  6. Technically a logograph might give some indication of pronunciation, whereas ideographs are purely indicative of a concept. It would seem that values under 100 are (or, at least in the original Latin, were) ideographs whereas 100 and over were originally logographs (e.g., C = centum, M = mille, but i ≠ unum, v ≠ quinque, x ≠ decem, l ≠ quinquaginta). Whether or not the author or printer was aware of this, however, is debatable.

  7. This contrasts with the “six and twenty” of 26,000 in line 353 (examined below), where the value would risk being misread as “twenty-six” if not spelled out.

  8. It is also the only ideograph rather than a logograph to represent a number over 50 (500 = Latin quingenti), which perhaps accounts for its rarity.

  9. Subitization is the ability to judge and report the number of items rapidly and accurately at a glance without the need to count them. The term was developed by Kaufman et al., who estimated that it occurs with almost complete confidence up to four items, relatively complete confidence up to six, adequately up to about eight, and as many as ten items could still be assessed within 2 and 4 s (1949, pp. 499–501).

  10. While much the same argument could be made for “fourtene” (l. 382) and “forty” (l. 217), both of these values occur at the beginning of a line, so any speculative discussion of them would be essentially moot.

  11. It must be added, however, that in three of the four examples here cited, the article occurs with a logographic realisation of the number.

  12. The only other number under 10 or 20 to occur is “seven”, which is also written out, but at the end of a line: “That made this worlde and dayes seuen” (l. 239).

  13. Such a structure would accord with Ford’s theory of the creation for formulae based on a prototype template (2002).

References

  • Amis and Amiloun (c. 1330). Edinburgh, National Library of Scotland, Advocates 19.2.1 (the Auchinleck Manuscript), fols. 49r–61v.

  • Amis and Amiloun (c. 1400). London, British Library, Egerton 2862 (olim Trentham-Sutherland), fols. 135r-147v.

  • Amis and Amiloun (c. 1500). Oxford, Bodleian Library, Douce 326, fols. 1r-13v.

  • Amis and Amiloun (c. 1500). London, British Library, Harley 2386, fols. 131r-137v.

  • Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M., & Burr, D. C. (2014). Separate mechanisms for perception of numerosity and density. Psychological Science, 25(1), 265–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M., & Burr, D. C. (2016). Number as a primary perceptual attribute: A review. Perception, 45, 5–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., & Coltheart, M. (1979). Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English. Neuropsychologia, 17, 467–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capystranus: A Metrical Romance (sine anno). Resource Document. Tudor Web of the Department of English and Linguistics, University of Otago. www.otago.ac.nz/english-linguistics/tudor/capystranus14649.html. Accessed 04 Dec 2017.

  • de Word, W. (1515). Capystranus. London, British Library, incunable C. 71. c. 26 (Short Title Catalogue no. 14649).

  • de Word, W. (1527). Capystranus. London, British Library, incunable C. 40. m. 9(18) (STC no. 14649.5).

  • de Word, W. (c. 1530). Capystranus. Oxford, Bodleian Library, Douce frag. f. 3 (STC no. 14650).

  • Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A. S. G. (1996). The reception of Malory’s Morte Darthur. In E. Archibald & A. S. G. Edwards (Eds.), Companion to Malory (pp. 241–252). Cambridge: D.S. Brewer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, J. (2002). Towards a new understanding of formulae: Prototypes and the mental template. Neuphilologische Mitteilungen, 103(2), 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fornaciai, M., & Park, J. (2017). Distinct neural signatures for very small and very large numerosities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 17, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, G. F. (1915). The development of Arabic numerals in Europe. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jevons, W. S. (1871/1911). Theory of political economy (4th edn.). London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, E. L., Lord, M. W., Reese, T. W., & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology, 62(4), 498–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masataka, N., Ohnishi, T., Imabayashi, E., Hirakata, M., & Matsuda, H. (2007). Neural correlates for learning to read Roman numerals. Brain and Language, 100, 276–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millar-Heggie, B. (2002). Sanctity, savagery and Saracens in Capystranus: Fifteenth-century Christian-Ottoman relations. Al-Masaq A Journal of the Medieval Mediterranean, 14(2), 113–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morillo, S. (1999). The battle of Hastings: Sources and interpretations. Woodbridge: Boydell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mount, T. (2016). A year in the life of medieval England (Kindle Edition ed.). The Hill, Stroud, Gloucestershire: Amberley Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norwich, J. J. (1982). A history of Venice. New York: Alfred B. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Picard, L. (2017). Chaucer’s people: Everyday lives in medieval England. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purdie, R. (2008). Anglicizing romance: Tail-rhyme and genre in medieval English literature. Cambridge: D.S. Brewer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Róna, É. (1966). Hungary in a medieval poem, Capystranus, a metrical romance. In M. Brahmer, S. Helsztynski & J. Krzyzanowski (Eds.), Studies in language and literature in honour of Margaret Schlauch (pp. 343–352). Warsaw: PWN Polish Scientific Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ropa, A. (2015). Imagining the 1456 siege of Belgrade in Capystranus. Hungarian Historical Review, 4(2), 255–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Setton, K. M. (1984). The sixteenth century to the reign of Julius III. Vol. 3 of the papacy and the Levant (1204–1571). Memoires of the American Philosophical Society 114. Philadelphia: The American Philosophical Society.

  • Shepherd, S. H. A. (Intro. and Ed.) (1995). Capystranus. In S. H. A. Shepherd (Ed.), Middle English romances (pp. 388–408). New York: Norton.

  • Smith, D. E. (1916). Review of G. F. Hill the development of Arabic numerals in Europe. Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, 22(4), 192–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verbruggen, J. F. (1997). The art of warfare in Western Europe during the Middle Ages. In S. Willard and R. W. Southern (Eds.), Woodbridge: Boydell.

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Bruno Baracat of the psychology section of the INU Champollion’s Department of Human and Social Science for guiding me towards appropriate neuropsychological studies in numerosity, numeracy and Weber’s Law of Just Noticeable Difference (difference threshold) for the completion of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John C. Ford.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ford, J.C. Two or III Feet Apart: Oral Recitation, Roman Numerals, and Metrical Regularity in Capystranus. Neophilologus 102, 573–593 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11061-018-9567-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11061-018-9567-7

Keywords

Navigation