Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Cognitome: Seeking the Fundamental Neuroscience of a Theory of Consciousness

  • Published:
Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Despite an enormous quantity of data, contemporary neuroscience does not provide satisfactory explanations for the nature of mind or consciousness. Today the main searches for answers to these questions focus around the problem of the neural bases of consciousness. This article analyzes the most important characteristics of consciousness and the requirements to be met by any fundamental scientific theory explaining it. These criteria are used to critique the most widely discussed neuroscience theories of consciousness. Among them are some satisfying some of the key requirements, though none meets all. I propose that a neuroscientific understanding of consciousness requires us to start thinking of the brain not as a connectome, i.e., a neural network, but as a neural hypernetwork consisting of neuronal groups with specific cognitive properties. The structure of the cognitome is the same as the structure of mind, while consciousness is a specific process of large-scale integration of cognitive elements in this neural hypernetwork.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Albantakis, L. and Tononi, G., “The intrinsic cause-effect power of discrete dynamical systems – from elementary cellular automata to adapting animats,” Entropy, 17, No. 8, 5472 (2015).

  • Albantakis, L., Hintze, A., Koch, C., et al., “Evolution of integrated causal structures in animats exposed to environments of increasing complexity, PLoS Comp. Biol., 10, No. 12, e1003966 (2014).

  • Allakhverdov, V. M., Consciousness as a Paradox, DNK, St. Petersburg (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, P. L. R., Darmaillacq, A.-S., Dennison, N., et al., “The identifi - cation and management of pain, suffering and distress in cephalopods, including anaesthesia, analgesia and humane killing,” J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol., 447, 46–64 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, K. V., “The cognitome: a hypernetwork model of the brain,” in: Proc. 17th All-Russ. Sci. Techn. Conf. Neuroinformatics-2015, Trofi - mov, A. G. (ed.) (2015), pp. 14–15.

  • Anokhin, K. V., “The cognitome: a theory of the realized degrees of freedom of the brain,” 5th Int. Conf. on Cognitive Science, Kaliningrad (2012), Vol. 2, pp. 420–430.

  • Anokhin, P. K., “Anticipatory refl ection of activity,” Vopr. Filos., 7, 91–111 (1962).

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K., “Basic questions in a general theory of functional systems,” in: Basic Systems Organization of Functions, Nauka, Moscow (1973), pp. 5–61.

  • Anokhin, P. K., “Dialectical materialism and questions of the ‘mental,’” Chelovek i Priroda, 1, 81–90 (1926).

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K., “Functional systems theory as a basis for understanding the compensatory processes of the body,” Uch. Zap. MGU Psikhol., 2, 32–41 (1947).

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K., “Key problems in studies of higher nervous activity,” in: Problems in Higher Nervous Activity, Moscow (1949), pp. 9–128.

  • Anokhin, P. K., “Systems genesis as a law of the evolutionary process,” Byull. Eksperim. Biol. Med., 8, No. 2, 81–99 (1948).

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K., “The role of the systems aspect in the development of problems at the junction of neurophysiology and psychology,” Bekhtereva, N. P. (ed.), Nauka, Leningrad (1974), pp. 10–16.

  • Baars, B., A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness, Cambridge University Press (1988).

  • Baars, B., In the Theater of Consciousness: The Workspace of the Mind, Oxford University Press, New York (1997).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolomei, F. and Naccache, L., “The global workspace (GW) theory of consciousness and epilepsy,” Behav. Neurol., 24, No. 1, 67–74 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. and Laland, K. N., “Tinbergen’s four questions: an appreciation and an update,” Trends Ecol. Evol., 28, No. 12, 712–718 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayne, T., “Access consciousness,” in: The Oxford Companion to Consciousness, Bayne, T., Cleeremans, A., and Wilken, P. (eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford (2009).

  • Bayne, T., Cleeremans, A., and Wilken, P. (eds.), The Oxford Companion to Consciousness, Oxford University Press, Oxford (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Beshkar, M., “The QBIT Theory of Consciousness,” Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci., 54, No. 4, 752–770 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Birch, J., Schnell, A. K., and Clayton, N. S., “Dimensions of animal consciousness,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 24, No. 10, 789–801 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N., “On a confusion about a function of consciousness,” Behav. Brain Sci., 18, No. 2, 227–247 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N., “Response to Kouider et al.: Which view is better supported by the evidence?” Trends Cogn. Sci., 16, No. 3, 141–142 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N., Consciousness, in: The Oxford Companion to the Mind, Gregory, R. L. (ed.), Oxford University Press, Oxford (2004).

  • Bolhuis, J. J. and Giraldeau, L.-A., “Introduction: Mechanisms of animal behaviour,” in: The Behavior of Animals: Mechanisms, Function and Evolution, Bolhuis, J. J., Giraldeau, L.-A. (eds.), Blackwell, Oxford (2005), pp. 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brenner, S., “Turing centenary: Life’s code script,” Nature, 482, No. 7386, 461 (2012).

  • Burghardt, G. M. and Bekoff, M., “Animal consciousness,” in: The Oxford Companion to Consciousness, Bayne, T., Cleeremans, A., and Wilken, P. (eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford (2009).

  • Carls-Diamante, S., “The octopus and the unity of consciousness,” Biol. Philos., 32, 1269–1287 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P., “Higher-order theories of consciousness,” in: The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness, Schneider, S. and Velmans, M. (eds.), John Wiley & Sons, Chichester (2017).

  • Casali, A. G., Gosseries, O., Rosanova, M., et al., “A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior,” Sci. Transl. Med., 5, No. 198, 198ra105 (2003).

  • Cerullo, M. A., “The problem with Phi: A critique of integrated information theory,” PLoS Comput. Biol., 11, No. 9, e1004286 (2015).

  • Chalmers, D. J., “Facing up to the problem of consciousness,” J. Consc. Stud., 2, 200–219 (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J., The Character of Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York (2010).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. J., The Conscious Mind: in Search of a Fundamental Theory, Oxford University Press, Oxford, (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., Martinez, R. M., and Cheng, Y., “The developmental origins of the social brain: Empathy, morality, and justice,” Front. Psychol., 9, 2584 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. A. and Dennett, D. C., “Consciousness cannot be separated from function,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 15, No. 8, 358–364 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. and Koch, C., “A framework for consciousness,” Nat. Neurosci., 6, No. 2, 119–126 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. and Koch, C., “Consciousness and neuroscience,” Cereb. Cortex, 8, 97–107 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. and Koch, C., “The unconscious homunculus,” Neuropsychoanalysis, 2, 2–59 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. and Koch, C., “Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness,” Seminars in Neurosci., 2, 263–275 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., “Neural Edelmanism,” Trends Neurosci., 12, No. 7, 240–248 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., “The astonishing hypothesis,” in: The Scientific Search for the Soul, Scribner, New York (1994).

  • Crook, R. J., Hanlon, R. T., and Walters, E. T., “Squid have nociceptors that display widespread long-term sensitization and spontaneous activity after bodily injury,” J. Neurosci., 33, No. 24, 10,021–10,026 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A., Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain, Pantheon Books, New York (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A., The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness, Harcourt Brace New York (1999).

  • Darwin, C., Charles Darwin’s Notebooks, 1836–1844, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1988).

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. and Preston, S. D., “Mammalian empathy: behavioural manifestations and neural basis,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 18, No. 8, 498–509 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. and Changeux, J. P., “Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing,” Neuron, 70, No. 2, 200–227 (2011).

  • Dehaene, S. and Changeux, J. P., “Ongoing spontaneous activity controls access to consciousness: a neuronal model for inattentional blindness,” PLoS Biol., 3, No. 5, e141 (2005).

  • Dehaene, S. and Naccache, L., “Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework,” Cognition, 79, No. 1–2, 1–37 (2001).

  • Dehaene, S., “Signatures of consciousness,” in: The Mind, Brockman, J. (ed.), Harper Perennial, New York (2011), pp. 217–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Consciousness and the Brain, Penguin Books (2015).

  • Dennett, D. C., Consciousness Explained, Little, Brown and Company, Boston (1991).

  • Descartes, R., Selected Works [Russian translation], State Political Literature Press, Moscow (1950).

  • Descartes, R., Works [Russian translation], Mysl’, Moscow, (1994), Vol. 2.

  • Dubrovskii, D. I. (ed.), The Problem of Consciousness in Philosophy and Science, Kanon+, Moscow (2009a).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubrovskii, D. I., “The problem of consciousness: experience in reviewing basic questions and theoretical difficulties,” in: Problems in Consciousness in Philosophy and Science, Dubrovskii, D. I. (ed.), Kanon+, Moscow (2009b), pp. 5–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubrovskii, D. I., The Consciousness and the Brain Problem: a Theoretical Solution, Kanon+, Moscow (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J., “A unitary hypothesis of mind–brain interaction in the cerebral cortex,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 240, No. 1299, 433–451 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, D. B. and Seth, A. K., “Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach,” Trends Neurosci., 32, 476–484 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, D. B., Baars, B. J., and Seth, A. K., “Identifying hallmarks of consciousness in non-mammalian species,” Conscious. Cogn., 14, No. 1, 169–187 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. and Gally, J. A.,“ Reentry: a key mechanism for integration of brain function, Front. Integr. Neurosci., 7, 63 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. and Tononi, G., Consciousness: How Matter Becomes Imagination, Allen Lane, London (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., “Group selection and phasic reentrant signaling: a theory of higher brain function,” in: The Mindful Brain: Cortical Organization and the Group-Selective Theory of Higher Brain Function, Edelman, G. M. and Mountcastle, V. B. (eds), MIT Press, Boston (1978), pp. 51–98.

  • Edelman, G. M., “Naturalizing consciousness: a theoretical framework,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 100, No. 9, 5520–5524 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., “Neural Darwinism: Selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function,” Neuron, 10, No. 2, 115–125 (1993).

  • Edelman, G. M., Bright Air, Brilliant Fire. On the Matter of the Mind, Basic Books, New York (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., Gally, J. A., and Baars, B. J., “Biology of consciousness,” Front. Psychol., 2, 4 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection, Basic Books, New York (1987).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., Second Nature. Brain Science and Human Knowledge, Yale University Press, New Haven (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., The Remembered Present: A Biological Theory of Consciousness, Basic Books, New York (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., Topobiology: An Introduction to Molecular Embryology, Basic Books, New York (1988).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., Wider than the Sky. The Phenomenal Gift of Consciousness, Yale University Press, New Haven (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • Einstein, A. and Infeld, L., The Evolution of Physics, The Scientific Book Club, London (1938).

    Google Scholar 

  • Einstein, A., Physics and Reality [Russian translation], Nauka, Moscow (1965).

    Google Scholar 

  • Engels, F., “The dialectics of nature,” in: K. Marx and F. Engels: Complete Works [Russian translation], Political Literature Press, Moscow (1961).

  • Fiorito, G. and Scotto, P., “Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris,” Science, 256, No. 5056, 545–547 (1992).

  • Frege, G., Thought. A Logical Investigation [Russian translation], Moscow (1997).

  • Ginsburg, S. and Jablonka, E., The Evolution of the Sensitive Soul: Learning and the Origins of Consciousness, MIT Press, Cambridge (2019).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P., Other Minds: The Octopus, the Sea and the Deep Origins of Consciousness, Farrar, Strauss and Giroux, neurotransmitter (2016).

  • Gray, J., Consciousness: Creeping up on the Hard Problem, Oxford University Press, Oxford (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D. R., The Question of Animal Awareness: Evolutionary Continuity of Mental Experience, Rockefeller University Press New York (1981).

  • Grossberg, S., “Adaptive Resonance Theory: how a brain learns to consciously attend, learn, and recognize a changing world,” Neural Netw., 37, 1–47 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Güntürkün, O. and Bugnyar, T., “Cognition without cortex,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 20, No. 4, 291–303 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gutfreund, Y., “The mind-evolution problem: the difficulty of fitting consciousness in an evolutionary framework,” Front. Psychol., 9, 1537 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Haig, D., “Proximate and ultimate causes: How come? and What for?” Biol. Philos., 28, 781–786 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. R. and Prouvost, C., “Jealousy in dogs,” PLoS One, 9, No. 7, e94597 (2014).

  • Hjelle, L. A. and Ziegler, D. J., Personality Theories: Basic Assumptions, Research, and Applications, McGraw-Hill Book Company, New York (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, T. and Schooler, J. W., “The easy part of the hard problem: A resonance theory of consciousness,” Front. Hum. Neurosci., 13, 378 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ivanitskii, A. M., “The cerebral basis of subjective experience: the information synthesis hypothesis,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 46, No. 2, 241–252 (1996).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • James, W., The Writings of William James: a Comprehensive Edition, University of Chicago Press, Chicago (1978).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemmerer, D., “Are we ever aware of concepts? A critical question for the global neuronal workspace, integrated information, and attended intermediate- level representation theories of consciousness,” Neurosci. Conscious, 2015, No. 1, niv006 (2015).

  • Keysers, C. and Gazzola, V., “A plea for cross-species social neuroscience,” Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci., 30, 179–191 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kingsbury, L., Huang, S., Wang, J., et al., “Correlated neural activity and encoding of behavior across brains of socially interacting animals,” Cell, 178, No. 2, 429–446.e16 (2019).

  • Koch, C., Consciousness. Confessions of a Romantic Reductionist, The MIT Press (2012).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., and Tononi, G., “Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 17, No. 5, 307–321 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C., The Feeling of Life Itself: Why Consciousness is Widespread but can’t be Computed, MIT Press, Cambridge (2019).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C., The Quest for Consciousness: a Neurobiological Approach, Roberts & Company Publishers, Englewood (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kouider, S., Sackur, J., and de Gardelle, V., “Do we still need phenomenal consciousness? Comment on Block,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 16, No. 3, 140–141 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuba, M. J., Byrne, R. A., Meisel, D. V., and Mather, J. A., “When do octopuses play? Effects of repeated testing, object type, age, and food deprivation on object play in Octopus vulgaris,” J. Comp. Psychol., 120, No. 3, 184–190 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamme, V. A. F., “Towards a true neural stance on consciousness,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 10, No. 11, 494–501 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laureys, S., Gosseries, O., and Tononi, G. (eds.), The Neurology of Consciousness, Elsevier, New York (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenoble, F. and Carlier, P., “A possible contribution of phenomenology to ethology: application to a behaviour pattern in the mouse,” Acta Biotheor., 44, No. 1, 75–83 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, J., “Materialism and qualia: the explanatory gap,” Pacif. Philos. Quart., 64, No. 4, 354–361 (1983).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J., Vidal, J. R., Fries, P., et al., “Selective neural synchrony suppression as a forward gatekeeper to piecemeal conscious perception,” Cereb. Cortex, 26, No. 7, 3010–3022 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mashour, G. A., Roelfsema, P., Changeux, J. P., and Dehaene, S., “Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis,” Neuron, 105, No. 5, 776–798 (2020).

  • Massimini, M. and Tononi, G., Sizing up Consciousness: Towards an Objective Measure of the Capacity for Experience, Oxford University Press, Oxford (2018).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Massimini, M., Ferrarelli, F., Murphy, M., et al., “Cortical reactivity and effective connectivity during REM sleep in humans,” Cogn. Neurosci., 1, No. 3, 176–183 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J. A. and Anderson, R. C., “Exploration, play and habituation in octopuses (Octopus dofleini),” J. Comp. Psychol., 113, 333 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J. A., “Cephalopod consciousness: behavioural evidence,” Conscious. Cogn., 17, No. 1, 37–48 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J., “What is in an octopus’s mind?” Anim. Sentience, 26, 1 (2019).

  • Mayr, E., “Cause and effect in biology,” Science, 134, 1501–1506 (1961).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, T. and Bayne, T., “Concepts, contents, and consciousness,” Neurosci. Conscious., 2016, No. 1, niv012 (2016).

  • McGinn, C., The Mysterious Flame: Conscious Minds in a Material World, Basic Books (2000).

  • Metzinger, T., Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity, MIT Press, Cambridge (2003).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T., The Ego Tunnel: The Science of the Mind and the Myth of the Self, Basic Books, New York (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Psychology: The Science of Mental Life, Harper & Row, New York (1962).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T., “What is it like to be a bat?” Philos. Rev., 83, No. 4, 435–450 (1974).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesher, N., Levy, G., Grasso, F. W., and Hochner, B., “Self-recognition mechanism between skin and suckers prevents octopus arms from interfering with each other,” Curr. Biol., 24, No. 11, 1271–1275 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M., “Tinbergen’s four questions: Two proximate, two evolutionary,” Evol. Med. Public Health, 2018, No. 1, 2 (2019).

  • Noel, J. P., Ishizawa, Y., Patel, S. R., et al., “Leveraging nonhuman primate multisensory neurons and circuits in assessing consciousness theory,” J. Neurosci., 39, No. 38, 7485–7500 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Noy, N., Bickel, S., Zion-Golumbic, E., et al., “Ignition’s glow: Ultra-fast spread of global cortical activity accompanying local “ignitions” in visual cortex during conscious visual perception,” Conscious. Cogn., 35, 206–224 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., and Tononi, G., “From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated information theory 3.0,” PLoS Comp. Biol., 10, No. 5, e1003588 (2014).

  • Panksepp, J. and Panksepp, J. B., “Toward a cross-species understanding of empathy,” Trends Neurosci., 36, No. 8, 489–496 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P., Complete Collection of Works, Academy of Sciences of the USSR Press, Moscow, Leningrad (1951), Vol. 2, Book 2.

  • Pavlov, I. P., Selected Works, Academy of Sciences of the USSR Press, Moscow, Leningrad (1949).

    Google Scholar 

  • Penrose, R., Shadows of the Mind. A Search for the Missing Science of Consciousness, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1994).

  • Ponomarev, Ya. A., A Methodological Introduction to Psychology, Nauka, Moscow (1983).

  • Sarasso, S., Boly, M., Napolitani, M., et al., “Consciousness and complexity during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine,” Curr. Biol., 25, No. 23, 3099–3105 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlicht, T., “Phenomenal consciousness, attention and accessibility,” Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci., 11, No. 3, 309–334 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, S., and Velmans, M. (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnell, A. K., Amodio, P., Boeckle, M., and Clayton, N. S., and “How intelligent is a cephalopod? Lessons from comparative cognition,” Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc., Epub.

  • Searle, J. R., “Can information theory explain consciousness?” New York Review of Books (2003), https://www.nybooks.com/articles/2013/01/10/can-information-theory-explain-0consciousness/.

  • Searle, J. R., “Consciousness,” Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 23, 557–578 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J., The Mystery of Consciousness, The New York Review of Books, New York (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, C. and Dehaene, S., “Neural processes underlying conscious perception: experimental fi ndings and a global neuronal workspace framework,” J. Physiol. (Paris), 98, No. 4–6, 374–384 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sergin, V. Ya., “Psychophysiological mechanisms of consciousness: the self-identification hypothesis,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 48, No. 3, 558–571 (1998).

  • Sergin, V. Ya., “Self-identification of neuron activity patterns as a physiological mechanism of consciousness,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 66, No. 2, 1–20 (2016).

  • Seth, A. K., Baars, B. J., and Edelman, D. B., “Criteria for consciousness in humans and other mammals,” Conscious. Cogn., 14, 119–139 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seth, A. K., Izhikevich, E., Reeke, G. N., and Edelman, G. M., “Theories and measures of consciousness: an extended framework,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 103, No. 28, 10,799–10,804 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shvyrkov, V. B., Introduction to Objective Psychology. The Neuronal Bases of the Mind. Selected Works, Aleksandrov, Yu. I. (ed.), Russian Academy of Sciences Institute of Psychology, Moscow (2006).

  • Sitt, J. D., King, J. R., Naccache, L., Dehaene, S., “Ripples of consciousness,” Trends Neurosci., 17, 552–554 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E. N., Essays on the Psychophysiology of Consciousness, Moscow State University, Moscow (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Spunt, R. P. and Adolphs, R., “The neuroscience of understanding the emotions of others,” Neurosci. Lett., 693, 44–48 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, N. S., The International Dictionary of Psychology, Crossroad, New York (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N., “On aims and methods of ethology,” Z. Tierpsychol., 20, 410–433 (1963).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G. and Edelman, G. M., “Consciousness and complexity,” Science, 282, No. 5395, 1846–1851 (1998).

  • Tononi, G. and Koch, C., “Consciousness: here, there and everywhere?” Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 370, No. 1668, 20140167 (2015).

  • Tononi, G., “An information integration theory of consciousness,” BMC Neurosci., 5, No. 1, 42 (2004).

  • Tononi, G., “Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto,” Biol. Bull., 215, No. 3, 216–242 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., “Integrated information theory of consciousness. Some ontological considerations,” in: The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness, Schneider, S. and Velmans, M. (eds.), John Wiley & Sons, Chichester (2017a), pp. 621–633.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., “Integrated information theory of consciousness: an updated account,” Arch. Ital. Biol., 150, No. 2–3, 56–90 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., “The integrated information theory of consciousness. An outline,” in: The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness, Schneider, S. and Velmans, M. (eds.), John Wiley & Sons, Chichester (2017b), pp. 243–256.

  • Tononi, G.,“ Why Scott should stare at a blank wall and reconsider (or the conscious grid) (2014), http://www.scottaaronson.com/blog/?p=1823.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., and Koch, C., “Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 17, No. 7, 450–461 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turing, A., “Computing machinery and intelligence,” Mind, LIX, No. 236, 433–460 (1950).

  • Van Bourg, J., Patterson, J. E., and Wynne, C. D. L., “Pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) release their trapped and distressed owners: Individual variation and evidence of emotional contagion,” PLoS One, 15, No. 4, e0231742 (2020).

  • Vasil’ev, V. V., The Hard Problem of Consciousness, Progress-Traditsiya, Moscow (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotskii, L. S., The Historical Means of the Crisis in Psychology, EKSMO-Press, Moscow.

  • Young, A. W., “Dissociable aspects of consciousness,” in: The Science of Consciousness: Psychological and Neuropsychological and Clinical Reviews, Velmans, M. (ed.), Routledge, London (2003), pp. 118–139.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to K. V. Anokhin.

Additional information

Translated from Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel’nosti imeni I. P. Pavlova, Vol. 71, No. 1, pp. 39–71, January–February, 2021.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Anokhin, K.V. The Cognitome: Seeking the Fundamental Neuroscience of a Theory of Consciousness. Neurosci Behav Physi 51, 915–937 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-021-01149-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-021-01149-4

Keywords

Navigation