Advertisement

Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology

, Volume 47, Issue 8, pp 987–993 | Cite as

Changes in the Ethogram in Rats Due to Contagion Behavior

  • D. G. Ivanov
  • N. A. Krupina
Article
  • 20 Downloads

Experiments were performed using a model of contagion behavior based on drinking behavior in rats. Observer rats without the drinking motivation, in the presence of a familiar conspecific with drinking motivation who demonstrated drinking behavior (demonstrator rats), displayed behavioral signs of the drinking motivation, i.e., signs of contagion behavior. Analysis of transition probability matrixes to investigate changes in the ethogram in observer rats demonstrated contagion behavior. Decreases in the frequency of aggressive and defensive behavioral patterns were seen in these animals, along with simultaneous increases in the frequency of exploratory activity, drinking, and exploratory approaches to bottles as compared with the ethograms of observer rats not displaying signs of contagion behavior on testing in the presence of a familiar conspecific demonstrator without the drinking motivation.

Keywords

ethogram social interaction contagion behavior transition probability matrixes rats 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altmann, S. A., “Avian mobbing behavior and predator recognition,” Condor, 58, 241–253 (1956).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barsade, S. G., “The ripple effect: emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior,” Admin. Sci. Q., 47, 644–675, (2002), doi:  https://doi.org/10.2307/3094912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Blanchard, R. J., Blanchard, D. C., and Takahashi, L. K., “Attack and defensive behaviour in the albino rat,” Anim. Behav., 25, 622–634 (1977).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Casarrubea, M., Sorbea, F., and Crescimanno, G., “Multivariate data handling in the study of rat behavior: An integrated approach,” Behav. Res. Meth., 41, No. 3, 772–781 (2009).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Casarrubea, M., Sorbera, F., Santangelo, A., and Crescimanno, G., “Learning influence on the behavioral structure of rat response to pain in hot-plate,” Behav. Brain Res., 225, No. 1, 177–183 (2011).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dickinson, A., “Goal-directed behavior and future planning in animals,” in: Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition, R. Menzel and J. Fischer (eds.), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (2011), pp. 81–93.Google Scholar
  7. Frolov, S. S., Sociology, Gardariki, Moscow (1999).Google Scholar
  8. Galef, B. G., “Imitation in animals. History, definition, and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory,” in: Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, Erlbaum Associates Publisher, Hillsdale, NJ (1988), pp. 3–28.Google Scholar
  9. Ivanov, D. G., Semenov, A. N., and Krupina, N. A., “A model of contagion behavior based on drinking behavior in rats,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 64, No. 6, 712–720 (2014).Google Scholar
  10. Lorenz, C., Aggression (so-called “evil”), Progress, Univers, Moscow (1994).Google Scholar
  11. Meaney, M. J. and Stewart, J., “Environmental factors influencing the affiliative behavior of male and female rats (Rattus norvegicus),” Anim. Learn. Behav., 7, No. 3, 397–405 (1979).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Miczek, K. A. and Boer, S. F., “Aggressive, defensive and submissive behavior,” in: The Behavior of the Laboratory Rat: a Handbook with Tests, Whishaw, I. Q. and Kolb, B. (eds.), Oxford University Press (2005), pp. 344–352.Google Scholar
  13. Moura, P. J., Meirelles, S. T., and Xavier, G. F., “Long-term social recognition memory in adult male rats: factor analysis of the social and non-social behaviors,” Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res., 43, 663–676 (2010).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Ol’shanskii, D. V., Psychological Mass, Pieter, St. Petersburg (2002).Google Scholar
  15. Reznikova, Zh. I., “Social learning in adults,” Priroda, No. 5, 3–12 (2009).Google Scholar
  16. Roberts, F. S., Discrete Mathematical Models with Application for Social, Biological, and Ecological Tasks, Nauka, Moscow (1986).Google Scholar
  17. Romero, T., Konno, A., and Hasegawa, T., “Familiarity bias and physiological responses in contagious yawning by dogs support link to empathy,” PLoS One, 8, No. 8, e71365.eCollection (2013), doi: 0.1371/journal.pone.0071365.Google Scholar
  18. Rosenthal, S. B., Twomey, C. R., Hartnett, A. T., et al., “Revealing the hidden networks of interaction in mobile animal groups allows prediction of complex behavioral contagion,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 112, No. 15, 4690–4695 (2015), doi:  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1420068112.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Singer, T. And Lamm, C. T., “The social neuroscience of empathy,” Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 1156, 81–96 (2009).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Vasconcelos, M., Hollis, K., Nowbahari, E., and Kacelnik, A., “Pro-sociality without empathy,” Biol. Lett., 8, 910–912 (2012).CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Wesson, D. W., “Sniffing behavior communicates social hierarchy,” Curr. Biol. 23, No. 7, 575–580 (2013).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Zentall, T. R., “Imitation in animals: Evidence, function and mechanisms,” Cybernetics and Systems. Int. J., 32, 53–96 (2001).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Zhmurov, V. A., Great Encyclopedia of Psychiatry, Dzhangar, Moscow (2010).Google Scholar
  24. Zimbardo, F. and Lyaippe, M., Social Influence, Pieter, St. Petersburg (2011).Google Scholar
  25. Zorina, Z. A., Poletaeva, I. I., and Reznikova, Zh. I., Basic Ethology and Behavioral Genetics, Moscow University Press, Moscow (2013).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Signal Science CenterMoscowRussia
  2. 2.Research Institute of General Pathology and PathophysiologyMoscowRussia

Personalised recommendations