Skip to main content
Log in

Delayed Copying of Unfamiliar Outline Images: Does the Decrease in Reaction Time with the Increase in the Delay Reflect a Change in the Internal Representation of the Forthcoming Movement?

  • Published:
Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A group of 24 adult subjects performed a task consisting of delayed motor reproduction (copying) of unfamiliar outline figures (trajectory templates). Templates were presented for 250 msec and were reproduced in response to an instruction given by sound signal (a brief click), delayed relative to the end of the period of presentation of the trajectory image by time T = 0, 500, 1000, 2000, or 4000 msec. Subjects performed the task in a block experiment in which the delay T was constant within each block of 32 trials. Reaction time (RT) was analyzed, along with the mean duration of movement along one segment of the trajectory (MT) and the duration of the pause between adjacent segments (DT). Reaction time RT was found to show a non-monotonic dependence on delay T, decreasing at T ≤ 1000 msec and increasing at T ≤ 2000 msec. The relationship between RT and T was adequately described by a very simple additive model – the sum of a linearly increasing component and an exponentially decreasing component. The linear increase reflected a decrease in the accuracy of predicting the moment at which the instruction signal would be presented as the delay T increased (foreperiod effect [Niemi and Näätänen, 1981; Meulenbroek and Van Galen, 1988]). The exponentially decreasing component is suggested to reflect transformation of the internal representation of the trajectory template during the period it is held in short-term memory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrahamse, E. L., Jimenez, L., Verwey, W. B., and Clegg, B. A., “Representing serial action and perception,” Psychon. Bull. Rev., 17, No. 5, 603–23 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Agam, Y., Huang, J., and Sekuler, R., “Neural correlates of sequence encoding in visuomotor learning,” J. Neurophysiol., 103, No. 3, 1418–24 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ashe, L., Taira, M., Smyrnis, N., et al., “Motor cortical activity preceding a memorized movement trajectory with an orthogonal bend,” Exp. Brain Res., 95, No. 1, 118–30 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Averbeck, B. B., Chafee, M. V., Crowe, D. A., and Georgopoulos, A. P., “Parallel processing of serial movements in prefrontal cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 99, No. 20, 13,172–13,177 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, A., Schöner, G., and Riehle, A., “Preshaping and continuous evolution of motor cortical representations during movement preparation,” Eur. J. Neurosci., 18, No. 7, 2047–58 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P. and Tisseyre, F., “The time-course of preparation with regular and irregular foreperiods,” Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 20, No. 3, 297–300 (1968).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boutin, A., Massen, C., and Heuer, H., “Modality-specific organization in the representation of sensorimotor sequences,” Front Psychol., 4, 93 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crammond, D. J. and Kalaska, J. F., “Prior information in motor and premotor cortex: activity during the delay period and effect on pre-movement activity,” J. Neurophysiol., 84, No. 2, 986–1005 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Desmurget, M., Pelisson, D., Rossetti, Y., and Prablanc, C. “From eye to hand: planning goal-directed movements,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 22, No. 6, 761–88 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erlhagen, W. and Schöner, G., “Dynamic field theory of movement preparation,” Psychol. Rev., 109, No. 3, 545–572 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holcombe, A. O., “Seeing slow and seeing fast: two limits on perception,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 13, No. 5, 216–221 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., et al., “The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation,” Psychol. Rev., 110, No. 2, 316–39 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Korneev, A. A. and Kurganskii, A. V., “The internal representation of a series of movements on reproduction of a static figure and the trajectory of a moving object,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 63, No. 4, 437–450 (2013).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, S. M., Jerde, T. A., Tzagarakis, C., et al., “Cerebellar activation during copying geometrical shapes,” J. Neurophysiol., 90, No. 6, 3874–87 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Los, S. A. and Agter, F., “Reweighting sequential effects across different distributions of foreperiods: Segregating elementary contributions to nonspecific preparation,” Percept. Psychophys., 67, No. 7, 1161–1170 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Los, S. A., Kruijne, W., and Meeter, M., “Outlines of a multiple trace theory of temporal preparation,” Front. Psychol., 5, 1058 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Machinskaya, R. I., Talalai, I. V., and Kurganskii, A. V., “Functional organization of the cerebral cortex in directed and implicit modally specific anticipatory attention. Analysis of the coherence of the alpha rhythm in the space of sources,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 65, No. 5, XXX (2015) (in press).

  • Meulenbroek, R. G. and Van Galen, G. P., “Foreperiod duration and the analysis of motor stages in a line-drawing task,” Acta Psychol. (Amst.), 69, No. 1, 19–34 (1988).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Niemi, P. and Näätänen, R., “Foreperiod and simple reaction time,” Psychol. Bull., 89, No. 1, 133–162 (1981).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, B. J., Bullock, D., Verwey, W. B., et al., “Learning and production of movement sequences: Behavioral, neurophysiological, and modeling perspectives,” Hum. Mov. Sci., 23, 699–746 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Cohen, R. G., Jax, S. A., et al., “The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley’s legacy,” Hum. Mov. Sci., 26, No. 4, 525–554 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swinnen, S. P., Vangheluwe, S., Wagemans, J., et al., “Shared neural resources between left and right interlimb coordination skills: the neural substrate of abstract motor representations,” Neuroimage, 49, No. 3, 2570–80 (2010).

  • Wong, A. L., Haith, A. M., and Krakauer, J. W., “Motor planning,” Neuroscientist., doi: 10.1177/1073858414541484 [Epub ahead of print] (2014).

  • Xu, Y. and Chun, M. M., “Selecting and perceiving multiple visual objects,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 13, No. 4, 167–174 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, E., Morrone, M. C., and Burr, D. C., “Buildup of spatial information over time and across eye movements,” Behav. Brain Res., 275, 281–287 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to A. A. Korneev.

Additional information

Translated from Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel’nosti imeni I. P. Pavlova, Vol. 66, No. 1, pp. 51–61, January–February, 2016.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Korneev, A.A., Lomakin, D.I. & Kurganskii, A.V. Delayed Copying of Unfamiliar Outline Images: Does the Decrease in Reaction Time with the Increase in the Delay Reflect a Change in the Internal Representation of the Forthcoming Movement?. Neurosci Behav Physi 47, 413–420 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-017-0414-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11055-017-0414-2

Keywords

Navigation