Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Allomorph selection precedes phonology: Evidence from Yindjibarndi

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theories of the phonology-morphology interface can be differentiated by their claims regarding the timing of phonologically conditioned suppletive allomorphy (PCSA) and phonology. Some (e.g. Paster 2006; Embick 2010) argue that PCSA occurs in a morphological component of the grammar that precedes phonology; others (e.g. Kager 1996; Mascaró 2007; Smith 2015) argue that at least phonologically optimizing PCSA occurs in the phonological component of the grammar, in parallel with phonology. This paper discusses a case of apparently optimizing PCSA in Yindjibarndi (Pama-Nyungan, Wordick 1982), proposes an analysis in which suppletive allomorphy precedes phonology, and shows that the alternative – an analysis in which PCSA occurs in the phonological component of the grammar – should be dispreferred.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Abbreviations: abl ablative case marker; dir.all direct allative case marker; emp emphatic clitic; erg ergative case marker; inst instrumental case marker; loc locative case marker; obj objective case marker; one one ; top topic clitic; voc vocative suffix.

  2. One might wonder if /ŋ/ contributes a mora to shorter words, allowing for the satisfaction of some minimal length requirement. This seems unlikely: stress in Biri appears to be quantity-insensitive (Terrill 1998:13), so there is no reason to think that [ŋ] (or [l]) is moraic.

  3. This paper relies on the grammatical description, text collection, and lexicon from Wordick (1982). I am grateful to the Yindjibarndi people who made his work possible. For another text collection, mixed with Ngarluma, see von Brandenstein (1970). For more wordlists, see Bowern (2016) and Maya Pilbara Aboriginal Language Centre (2003). For work on the acoustics of Yindjibarndi nasal and stop consonants, see Tabain (1994) and Tabain and Butcher (1999).

  4. I use binary place features here because it allows me to easily make a distinction between coronal and non-coronal consonants, which will become relevant later in the paper.

  5. Wordick’s (1982) lexicon lists a total of 13 vowel-initial morphemes; seven are suffixes and the rest are English loans.

  6. Throughout this paper, numbers in parentheses refer to page numbers in Wordick’s (1982) grammar.

  7. Note that assuming this architecture of the grammar predicts that suppletive allomorphy cannot be conditioned by derived phonology. There are no such cases in Yindjibarndi that I am aware of.

  8. An anonymous reviewer points out several words on the Yindjibarndi 50 Words site (https://50words.online/) that suggest cluster reduction from NC to C happens in other contexts, as well: /paŋgari/ ‘where are you going’ sounds like it is realized as [pagari], and /gurumau/ ‘goanna’ sounds like it is realized as [gurumat̪u]. Further investigation of a larger corpus of spoken Yindjibarndi is necessary to draw conclusions regarding additional contexts for nasal cluster reduction.

  9. I believe that *NCV(C)NC is shorthand for a phonetically motivated constraint that penalizes nasal-stop sequences that are followed by nasal or nasalized vowels. For motivation and formalization, as well as an analysis of the Yindjibarndi facts along these lines, see Stanton (2019).

  10. Presumably, these constraints interact with other constraints on segment sequencing, for example markedness constraints against vocalic sequences. Nash (2017) has shown, for example, that iCu sequences are avoided in many Pama-Nyungan languages.

  11. Wordick characterizes /k/ lenition as applying only to morpheme-initial segments, but I was unable to find an intervocalic [k] or [w] in any of the suffixes in his lexicon. In addition, it is potentially worth noting that there is a broader dispreference for intervocalic /k/s in the language. /k/ is a frequent consonant in Yindjibarndi: of the 7,604 consonants in Wordick’s lexicon (n = 2,273 words, entered by hand), 714 of these, or 9.39%, are /k/. (The only more frequent consonants are /m/, with 723 occurrences, and /r/, with 799.) If all consonants were equally probable in all environments, we might then expect 9.39% of all VCV sequences to involve a /k/. But what we find is that only 2.05% (66/3218) do. Thus although /k/ is frequent, it is underattested between vowels; the observed/expected ratio in this environment is 0.21 (=66/3218*.0939); see Pierrehumbert 1992 on O/E). It is likely, then, that (20) reflects a broader trend in the language.

  12. For another resource that could be used to calculate lexical statistics in Yindjibarndi, see Chirila (Bowern 2016).

  13. Recall that three Max constraints were actually employed in Sect. 3: Max, Max[stem], and Max[-sonorant]. Max[stem] is irrelevant because the only repairs I consider here affect the suffix. It does not matter whether the constraint I discuss is Max or Max[-sonorant]; all that is crucial for the present argument is that one of them dominates *LateCC.

  14. Note that, by virtue of using Harmonic Serialism, the analysis in Sect. 4.1 differs from one that would come from Wolf (2008), who uses Optimality Theory with Candidate Chains (OT-CC, McCarthy 2007). All analytical points raised in this section also apply to an OT-CC-based analysis.

  15. The emphatic clitic is /pa/. We can tell that this is a different morpheme than /mpa/ because the /p/ of emphatic /pa/ lenites, but the /p/ of topicalization /mpa/ does not (compare /munti+mpa/ → [munti-pa] ‘truly-top’ to /munti+pa/ → [munti-wa] ‘truly-emp’ (Wordick 1982:225)). For clarity, I assume that /mpa/ is a non-leniting suffix, like /puɳu/ ‘user’ (Wordick 1982:116). I have not been able to find an example of the emphatic attaching to a [r]-final word to confirm that the morpheme-initial /p/ lenites there too, but this is what is expected given Wordick’s description.

  16. The associate editor notes that the analysis could also be made to work by assuming that final consonants are not parsed into feet, and that the subcategorization requirements of /ŋka/ require an immediately preceding foot boundary.

References

  • Alderete, John. 1997. Dissimilation as local conjunction. In Proceedings of the North East Linguistics Society, ed. Kiyomi Kusumoto, Vol. 27, 17–32. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alderete, John. 2008. Using learnability as a filter on factorial typology: A new approach to Anderson and Browne’s generalization. Lingua 118: 1177–1220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, Jill N. 1998. Positional faithfulness. PhD diss., UMass Amherst, Amherst, MA.

  • Blevins, Juliette. 2004. Evolutionary phonology: The emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blust, Robert. 2012. One mark per word? Some patterns of dissimilation in Austronesian and Australian languages. Phonology 29: 355–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, Paul. 2003. Review of “Learnability in optimality theory” (Bruce Tesar and Paul Smolensky, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press). Phonology 20: 436–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonet, Eulàlia, Maria-Rosa Lloret, and Joan Mascaró. 2007. Allomorph selection and lexical preferences: Two case studies. Lingua 117: 1903–1927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowern, Claire. 2016. Chirila: Contemporary and historical resources for the indigenous languages of Australia. Language Documentation & Conservation 10: 1–44.

  • Bye, Patrik. 2008. Allomorphy – selection, not optimization. In Freedom of analysis?, eds. Sylvia Blaho, Patrik Bye, and Martin Krämer, 63–92. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstairs, Andrew. 1988. Some implications of phonologically conditioned suppletion. In Yearbook of morphology 1988, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 67–94. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam, and Morris Halle. 1968. The sound pattern of English. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dench, Alan Charles. 1995. Martuthunira: A language of the Pilbara region of Western Australia, Vol. 125. Canberra: Australian National University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, David. 2010. Localism and globalism in morphology and phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gouskova, Maria, Luiza Newlin-Lukowicz, and Sofya Kasyanenko. 2015. Selectional restrictions as phonotactics over sublexicons. Lingua 167: 41–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halle, Morris, and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The view from building 20. Essays in honor of Sylvain Bromberger, eds. Kenneth Hale and Samuel Jay Keyser, 111–176. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, Philip James. 1995. Constraints and markedness in the phonotactics of Australian Aboriginal languages. PhD diss., University of Toronto, Toronto.

  • Harrell, Richard S. 1962. A short reference grammar of Moroccan Arabic. Washington: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinz, Jeffrey. 2009. On the role of locality in learning stress patterns. Phonology 26: 303–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, Robert K. 1986. Language universals, markedness theory, and natural phonetic processes. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Inkelas, Sharon. 2014. The interplay of morphology and phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, Caroline. 2000. Licit vs. illicit responses in Meinhof’s Rule phenomena. In MIT working papers in linguistics, Vol. 37, 95–103. Cambridge: MITWPL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kager, René. 1996. On affix allomorphy and syllable counting. In Interfaces in phonology, ed. Ursula Kleinhenz, 155–171. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalin, Laura. 2020. Morphology before phonology: A case study of Turoyo (Neo-Aramaic). Morphology 30: 135–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalin, Laura, and Nicholas Rolle. 2020. Deconstructing subcategorization: Conditions on insertion versus position. Talk presented at the 51th Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society (slides available at https://sites.grenadine.uqam.ca/sites/linguistique/en/nels51/documents/get_document/224).

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1973. “Elsewhere” in phonology. In A festschrift for Morris Halle, eds. Stephen Anderson and Paul Kiparsky, 93–106. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1996. Allomorphy or morphophonology? In Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science series, 13–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, Thomas B. 2003. Syllable structure and lexical markedness in creole morphophonology: Determiner allomorphy in Haitian and elsewhere. In Phonology and morphology of Creole languages, ed. Ingo Plag, 209–228. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, Harold J. 1980. Kaititj nominal inflection: Some comparative notes. In Papers in Australian Linguistics 13: Contributions to Australian Linguistics, eds. Bruce Rigsby and Peter Sutton, 259–274. Canberra: Australian National University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurisu, Kazutaka. 2001. The phonology of morpheme realization. PhD diss., University of California, Santa Cruz, CA.

  • Mascaró, Joan. 2007. External allomorphy and lexical representation. Linguistic Inquiry 38: 715–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maya Pilbara Aboriginal Language Centre, Wangka. 2003. Yindjibarndi: Yindjibarndi-English Dictionary, English-Yindjibarndi Dictionary and Topical Wordlist. South Hedland: Wangka Maya.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2007. Hidden generalizations: Phonological opacity in optimality theory. Sheffield: Equinox Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2008. The gradual path to cluster simplification. Phonology 25: 271–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J. 2010. An introduction to harmonic serialism. Language and Linguistics Compass 4(10): 1001–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1993a. Generalized alignment. In Yearbook of Morphology 1993, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 79–153. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1993b. Prosodic morphology I: Constraint interaction and satisfaction. Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst and Rutgers University.

  • McConvell, Patrick. 1988. Nasal cluster dissimilation and constraints on phonological variables in Gurundji and related languages. Aboriginal Linguistics 1: 135–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, David. 2017. ‘*iCu’ in Australia: Phonotactics beyond the syllable level. Poster presentation at the 12th Association for Linguistic Typology (ALT2017). Available at https://www.anu.edu.au/linguistics/nash/papers/2017-ALT-iCu-A1.pdf.

  • O’Hara, Charles Patrick. 2021. Soft biases in phonology: Learnability meets grammar. PhD diss., University of Southern California.

  • Paster, Mary. 2006. Phonological conditions on affixation. PhD diss., University of California, Berkeley, CA.

  • Paster, Mary. 2014. Allomorphy. In The Oxford handbook of derivational morphology, eds. Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Štekauer, 219–234. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paster, Mary. 2016. Diachronic sources of allomorphy. In The morphosyntax-phonology connection: Locality and directionality at the interface, eds. Vera Gribanova and Stephanie S. Shih, 419–443. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1992. Dissimilarity in the Arabic Verbal Roots. In Proceedings of the 23rd meeting of the Northeastern Linguistic Society. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 2004. Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Research Unit for Indigenous Language at the University of Melbourne. 2021. 50 Words Project. Available at http://www.50words.online.

  • Sands, Kristina. 1996. The ergative in Proto-Australian. München: LINCOM Europa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, Brian W. 2015. Phonologically-conditioned allomorphy and UR constraints. PhD diss., University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA.

  • Stanton, Juliet. 2016. Learnability shapes typology: The case of the midpoint pathology. Language 92: 753–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, Juliet. 2019. Constraints on contrast drive nasal cluster dissimilation. Phonology 36: 655–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabain, Marija. 1994. A spectrographic study of nasal consonants in Yanyuwa and Yindjibarndi. Master’s thesis, University of Melbourne, Melbourne.

  • Tabain, Marija, and Andrew R. Butcher. 1999. Stop consonants in Yanyuwa and Yindjibarndi: Locus equation data. Journal of Phonetics 27(4): 333–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrill, Angela. 1998. Biri. München: LINCOM Europa.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Brandenstein, Carl Georg. 1970. Narratives from the north-west of Western Australia in the Ngarluma and Jindjiparndi languages. Acton: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, Matthew Adam. 2008. Optimal interleaving: Serial phonology-morphology interaction in a constraint-based model. PhD diss., University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA.

  • Wordick, Frank. 1982. The Yindjibarndi Language. Canberra: Australian National University.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

My thanks to Adam Albright, Arto Anttila, Maria Gouskova, Kate Mooney, Donca Steriade, three anonymous reviewers, and audiences at NYU’s Morphbeer, LSA 93, and Berkeley’s Phorum for comments on and critical discussion of this material.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Juliet Stanton.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stanton, J. Allomorph selection precedes phonology: Evidence from Yindjibarndi. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 40, 1317–1352 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-021-09531-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-021-09531-0

Keywords

Navigation