Skip to main content
Log in

Minimality, maximality and perfect prosodic word in Alcozauca Mixtec

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In Alcozauca Mixtec, an Otomanguean language spoken in Mexico, the prosodic word is ideally minimally and maximally bimoraic. On the one hand, all monosyllabic stems have a long vowel so that they would be bimoraic, which has been reported across Mixtec varieties. On the other hand, prosodic words that are longer than two moras often undergo truncation and allomorphic alternation to fit into this ideal template that consists of a bimoraic foot, especially in casual speech. In this sense, the bimoraic foot constitutes the perfect prosodic word (Ito and Mester 2015) in Alcozauca Mixtec. A set of prosodic-word size restrictor constraints, namely Foot-Binarity, Parse(μ) and All-Feet-Right, is proposed to account for minimality effects (McCarthy and Prince 1994:17); in this paper, it will be shown that maximality effects are also predicted by the same set of constraints, and that such a prediction is borne out in Alcozauca Mixtec. Such an analysis is contrasted with an analysis with a constraint against a non-head foot, *Ft- (de Lacy 2003), with an analysis with a cover constraint Perfect Word (Ito and Mester 2015), and with an economy constraint *Structure (Prince and Smolensky 1993); it will be argued that a reductionist approach with the three prosodic-word size restrictor constraints is more appropriate to account for the Mixtec data. Such an approach also makes a typological prediction that maximality always entails minimality, but not vice versa.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The small capital l represents that the word begins with a low tone. A high tone is assigned in the surface form from the second mora when the word has no accent.

  2. In Alcozauca Mixtec, the violation of this constraint is evaluated by one violation for each foot that is not at the right edge of the PWrd regardless of how many intervening constituents there are.

  3. In the original definition in Snider (2018), tone patterns are properties of the morpheme, and not prosodic structures such as the foot. In this paper, we loosen the definition to include the non-basic tone patterns that are realized on the foot, which may result from morpheme concatenation, such as ka2sũ1 [prf.fry] ‘fried’. We include such forms because some of the tonal patterns are only found in polymorphemic forms in Alcozauca Mixtec, admitting that the foot is the prosodic structure to which the complex morphosyntactic structure is mapped. For the same reason, the quadruplet in (11) contains forms from different lexical categories.

  4. Penner (2019:86) lists two phonotactic constraints motivated by the Obligatory Contour Principle (the prohibition against sequences of a glide + their homorganic vowels, *ji, *wo, *wu) as the justification for the syllable, since the syllable is its domain.

  5. In this paper, we use the term ‘stem’ to refer to the constituent made up of the verb root plus realis or irrealis prefix (which may or may not be analyzable synchronically), following Macaulay (1996).

  6. An anonymous reviewer pointed out that this word may have come into Mixtec via Nahuatl (Nordell 1984:10).

  7. It is not the case that all original SVPV sequences have undergone syncope and prothesis in Alcozauca Mixtec; there are forms such as si3ti2 ‘intestines’, ʃi1ko1 ‘smell’. This difference may be because this sound change is in progress: some forms have already undergone these processes while others have not.

  8. This syncope appears to be barely motivated. It is syncope of /a/ (or /i/), a sonorous vowel, in the tonic syllable (in the case of disyllabic forms), which is unexpected (Gouskova 2003:Ch. 4). An alternative to *SVP is to attribute syncope to general economy constraint *V (Hartkemeyer 2000) in conjunction with a constraint limiting the onset to a sibilant-plosive sequences, which is crosslinguistically common (Goad 2011). However, as Gouskova (2003:Ch. 2) points out, and as will be discussed in Sect. 6.3, economy constraints such as *V should be excluded from the Con. In addition, as was stated in the previous footnote, since not all the forms with the SVPV sequence have undergone syncope + prothesis, *SVP is best considered an indexed constraint (Bye 2007).

  9. This constraint is a combination of Max-IO(C), which requires that the input consonants are maximized in the output and Ident-IO(CF) which requires that output correspondents of consonants have identical feature values. Violation of the Max-IO(C) component of this cover constraint in the root necessarily entails violation of Maxroot, as in (31c) and (31d).

  10. An anonymous reviewer pointed out that in some varieties such as in Ixpantepec Nieves Mixtec there are forms such as intya̱a̱n ‘tomorrow’ (Carroll 2015:112), where the pre-foot syllable with no onset does not undergo syncope, even though the resulting onset cluster would be licit and thus syncope is expected with this ranking. However, its cognate in Alcozauca Mixtec has indeed lost the pre-foot syllable: ca1ã1 ‘tomorrow’, thus supporting the ranking here. There are still very few cases of pre-foot, word-initial vowels as in i3sa4a3 ‘day after tomorrow’, the initial vowel of which cannot be elided even in casual speech; at this point we do not know the factors which determine the maintenance and deletion of such vowels.

  11. The difference from accounting for the allomorphy by postulating only ni1- in the input with the deletion of segments in certain contexts is that in such an analysis the allomorph Ø1- would incur violation of Max, while listing both allomorphs in the input does not violate this constraint, even when the zero allomorph is selected.

  12. An anonymous reviewer wondered if a more general Maxroot constraint eliminates such a candidate. However, in Sect. 4 it was established that Maxroot dominates Parse(μ) in careful speech while the ranking is reversed in casual speech. This predicts that candidate (62c) would be optimal in casual speech, which is not the case.

  13. When the stem ends in a tone other than tone 4, as in (72), this stem tone is deleted when a monomoraic diphthong is formed, rather than forming a contour tone; generally, contour tones are restricted to the first mora of the foot in Alcozauca Mixtec (Sect. 3.2). This fact also supports the analysis that oe is monomoraic, rather than bimoraic.

  14. Tighter integration of vowel-initial postposed morphemes, in contrast to the exclusion of consonant-initial morphemes, is also reported in other languages, such as in Tani languages (Post 2017), Catalan, and Alemanic dialects (Bergmann 2018; Caro Reina 2019:Ch. 2).

  15. According to Anderson (2005:55), the difference between the structures in (84a) and (84b) is due the different rankings of constraints responsible for the organization into prosodic constituents: NonRecursive(PWd), Exhaustivity(PWd) » Prosodic Faithfulness for (84a) and Prosodic Faithfulness » Exhaustivity(PPh) » NonRecursive(PWd) for (84b). Thus, the difference between the V-initial and C-initial enclitics could be accounted for by two Cophonologies which differ in the constraint rankings. Alternatively, assuming that an onsetless syllable is dispreferred except for in the foot-medial position (expressed here as Onset), and that this constraint prohibits the V-initial enclitics from forming the structure in (84b), the behaviors of both types of enclitics may be captured by a single ranking with the ranking Onset dominating other constraints.

  16. Here, following the suggestion made by an anonymous reviewer, we assume that diphthongs and glide formation are distinct. In a diphthong, both vowels are associated with a mora, although it might be a single shared mora, while in glide formation, the vowel is no longer associated with a mora (cf. Rosenthall 1994, 1997).

  17. An alternative is to consider that this is due to Onset, which requires onset in all syllables. Employing such a constraint would require a positional faithfulness such as Ident-σ1(F) (Beckman 1998:56), in this case preventing the word-initial syllables to delete a vowel or insert a consonant to achieve the ideal CV structure (cf. Casali 1997; Beckman 1998), since word-initial onset-less syllables are allowed. Here we opt for NoHiatus, which is understood to be a conjunction of Onset and a positional faithfulness constraint.

  18. An anonymous reviewer points out that this constraint does not reflect the frequency and co-occurrence or distributional asymmetries found in modern varieties, which favor the ‘outer triangle’ vowels (cf. Sect. 3.1), and that it does not fit the trend in Mixtec sound change for ‘inner triangle’ vowels to merge with ‘outer triangle’ vowels described in Josserand (1983:270–272). However, a coherent synchronic account is hard to achieve without a constraint Max-Vmid.

References

  • Anderson, Stephen R. 2005. Aspects of the theory of clitics. Oxford studies in theoretical linguistics. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Backley, Phillip. 2011. An introduction to element theory. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, Jill. 1998. Positional faithfulness. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Benua, Laura. 1997. Transderivational identity: Phonological relations between words. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Bergmann, Pia. 2018. The phonological word in German: Insights from an acoustic-phonetic study of complex words. In Empirical approaches to the phonological structure of words, eds. Christiane Ulbrich, Alexander Werth, and Richard Wiese, 13–44. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bickford, J. Albert, and Stephen A. Marlett. 1988. The semantics and morphology of Mixtec mood and aspect. Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota 32: 1–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broselow, E. 2009. Stress adaptation in loanword phonology: Perception and learnability. In Phonology in perception, eds. Paul Boersma and Silke Hamann, 191–234. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bye, Patrik. 2007. Allomorphy: Selection, not optimization. In Freedom of analysis? eds. Sylvia Blaho, Patrik Bye, and Martin Krämer, 63–91. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro Reina, Javier. 2019. Central Catalan and Swabian: A study in the framework of the typology of syllable and word languages. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, Lucien Serapio. 2015. Ixpantepec nieves Mixtec word prosody. Ph.D. dissertation, UC San Diego.

  • Casali, Roderic. 1997. Vowel elision in hiatus contexts. Which vowel goes? Language 73: 493–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casali, Roderic. 2011. Hiatus resolution. In Ch. 61, The Blackwell companion to phonology, eds. Marc Van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice, 1434–1460. London: Blackwell Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castillo García, Rey. 2007. Descripción fonológica segmental y tonal del Mixteco de Yoloxóchitl, Gro. MA thesis, Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores de Antropología Social.

  • Clark, Lawrence E. 1977. Linguistic acculturation in Sayula Popoluca. International Journal of American Linguistics 43(2): 128–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, G. N., Alexis Michaud, and Cédric Patin. 2010. Do we need tone features? In Tones and features: Phonetic and phonological perspectives. Vol. 107 of Studies in generative grammar, eds. John Goldsmith, Elizabeth Hume, and Leo Wetzels, 3–24. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, Lisa, and Rolf Noyer. 1996. Loan phonology in Huave: Nativization and the ranking of faithfulness constraints. In West coast conference on formal linguistics, eds. Brian Agbayani and Sze-Wing Tang. Vol. 15 of WCCFL, 65–80. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiCanio, Christian, Joshua Benn, and Rey Castillo García. 2018. The phonetics of information structure in Yoloxóchitl Mixtec. Journal of Phonetics 68: 50–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerfen, Chip. 1999. Phonology and phonetics in Coatzospan Mixtec. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gittlen, Laura, and Stephen A. Marlett. 1985. Numi Mixtec syllable structure and morphology. In Workpapers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota, 175–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goad, Heather. 2011. The representation of sC clusters. In Ch. 38, The Blackwell companion to phonology, eds. Marc Van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice, 898–923. London: Blackwell Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, John. 1990. Autosegmental and metrical phonology. Oxford: Blackwell Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouskova, Maria. 2003. Deriving economy: Syncope in optimality theory. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Hall, Nancy. 2011. Vowel epenthesis. In Ch. 67, The Blackwell companion to phonology, eds. Marc Van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice, 1576–1596. London: Blackwell Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartkemeyer, Dale C. 2000. *V: An optimality-theoretic examination of vowel loss phenomena, with special reference to Latin, early Western Romance, and Basque, Ph.D. dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

  • Hayes, Bruce, Bruce Tesar, and Kie Zuraw. 2013. OTSoft 2.5, software package, http://www.linguistics.ucla.edu/people/hayes/otsoft/. Last accessed 22 April 2021.

  • Hollenbach, B. E. 2003. The historical source of an irregular Mixtec tone-sandhi pattern. In Language and life: Essays in memory of Kenneth L. Pike, eds. Mary Ruth Wise, Thomas N. Headland, and Ruth M. Brend, 533–550. Dallas: SIL International and University of Texas at Arlington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, Brett. 2012. Alignment constraints. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 30(3): 789–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, Larry. 2010. Do tones have features? In Tones and features: Phonetic and phonological perspectives. Vol. 107 of Studies in generative grammar, eds. John Goldsmith, Elizabeth Hume, and Leo Wetzels, 50–80. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inkelas, Sharon, and Cheryl Zoll. 2007. Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned phonology. Linguistics 45(1): 133–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Junko, and Armin Mester. 1995. Japanese phonology. In Handbook of phonological theory, ed. John Goldsmith, 817–838. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2009. The extended prosodic word. In Phonological Domains. Universals and Deviations, eds. Baris Kabak and Janet Grijzenhout, 135–194. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2015. The perfect prosodic word in Danish. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 38(1): 5–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josserand, Judy Kathryn. 1983. Mixtec dialect history. Ph.D. dissertation, Tulane University.

  • Kager, René. 1995. On foot templates and root templates. In Linguistics in the Netherlands 1995, eds. Marcel den Dikken and Kees Hengeveld, 125–138. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kager, René. 1999. Optimality theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, Yoonjung. 2011. Loanword phonology. In Ch. 95, The Blackwell companion to phonology, eds. Marc Van Oostendorp, Colin J. Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice, 2258–2282. London: Blackwell Sci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ketner, Katherine Heidel. 2006. Size restrictions in prosodic morphology. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Cambridge.

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 2000. Opacity and cyclicity. The Linguistic Review 17: 351–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 2015. Phonologization. In Handbook of historical phonology, eds. Patrick Honeymoon and Joseph Salmons. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurisu, Kazutaka. 2001. The phonology of morpheme realization. Ph.D. dissertation, UC Santa Cruz.

  • de Lacy, Paul. 2003. Maximal words and the Maori passive. In Austronesian Formal Linguistics Association, ed. Norvin Richards (AFLA 8, 20–39). Cambridge: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 44.

    Google Scholar 

  • León Vázquez, Octavio. 2017. Sandhi tonal en el Mixteco de Yucuquimi de Ocampo. MA tesis, Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios en Antropología Social.

  • Lin, Hui-Shan. 2018. Vowel hiatus resolution in Kavalan. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics 16(1): 53–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardi, Linda. 2003. Markedness and the typology of epenthetic vowels. Ms., University of Maryland at College Park, ROA-578.

  • Longacre, Robert E. 1957. Proto-Mixtecan. International Journal of American Linguistics 23(4): 1–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macaulay, Monica, and Joseph C. Salmons. 1995. The phonology of glottalization in Mixtec. International Journal of American Linguistics 61(1): 38–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macaulay, Monica. 1996. A grammar of Chalcatongo Mixtec. Vol. 127 of University of California publications in linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macken, Marlys A., and Joseph C. Salmons. 1997. Prosodic templates in sound change. Diachronica 14(1): 31–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlett, Stephen. 1992. Nasalization in Mixtec languages. International Journal of American Linguistics 58: 425–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1993. Generalized alignment. In Yearbook of morphology, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap Marle, 79–153. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1994. The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in prosodic morphology, ROA- 13. Rutgers optimality archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Papers in Optimality Theory, eds. Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, and Suzanne Urbancyck. Vol. 18 of University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics. Amherst, Mass: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1986/1996. Prosodic morphology. Technical report #32, Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. Ms., http://works.bepress.com/John_j_mccarthy/54/. Last accessed 22 April 2021.

  • McKendry, Inga. 2013. Tonal Association, Prominence and prosodic structure in south-eastern Nochixtlán Mixtec. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Edinburgh.

  • McPherson, Laura. 2017. Tone features revisited: Evidence from Seenku (Mande, Burkina Faso). In Diversity in African languages: Selected papers from the 46th annual conference on African linguistics, eds. Doris Payne, Sara Pacchiarotti, and Mokaya Bosire, 5–21. Berlin: Language Science Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendoza Ruiz, Juana. 2016. Fonología segmental y patrones tonales del Tu’un Savi de Alcozauca de Guerrero. MA thesis, Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios en Antropología Social.

  • Nespor, Marina, and Irene Vogel. 1986/2007. Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nordell, Norman. 1984. Spanish loan words via Aztec. SIL-Mexico Workpapers 5: 9–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palancar, Enrique, Jonathan Amith, and Rey Castillo García. 2016. Verbal inflection in Yoloxóchitl Mixtec. In Tone and inflection, eds. Enrique Palancar and Jean Léo Léonard, 295–336. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paster, Mary. 2006. Phonological conditions on affixation. Ph.D dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.

  • Pater, Joe. 1997. Minimal violation and phonological development. Language Acquisition 6: 201–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penner, Kevin L. 2019. Prosodic structure in Ixtayutla Mixtec: Evidence for the foot. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Alberta.

  • Piñeros, Carlos-Eduardo. 2000. Prosodic and segmental unmarkedness in Spanish truncation. Linguistics 38(1): 63–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, Kenneth. 1944. An analysis of a Mixteco text. International Journal of American Linguistics 10(4): 113–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, Kenneth. 1947. Grammatical prerequisites to phonemic analysis. Word 3: 155–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, Kenneth. 1948. Tonemic perturbations in Mixteco, with special emphasis on tonomechanical subclasses. In Ch. 7: Tone languages: A technique for determining the number and type of pitch contrasts in a language, with studies in tonemic substitution and fusion, 77–94. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poser, William. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66: 78–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Post, Mark W. 2017. The Tangam language: Grammar, lexicon and texts. Leiden: Brill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan 1980. A metrical theory for Estonian quantity. Linguistic Inquiry 11(3): 511–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality theory. Rutgers: University and University of Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthall, Samuel. 1994. Vowel/glide alternation in a theory of constraint interaction. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Rosenthall, Samuel. 1997. The distribution of prevocalic vowels. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 15(1): 139–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubach, J., and G. E. Booij. 2001. Allomorphy in optimality theory: Polish iotation. Language 77: 26–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sande, Hannah. 2018. Cross-word morphologically conditioned scalar tone shift in Guébie. Morphology 28: 253–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1984. Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1995. The prosodic structure of function words. In Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, Vol. 187, 214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, Elizabeth. 2011. Syntax-phonology interface, 2nd edn. In The handbook of phonological theory, ed. John Goldsmith, 435–484. London: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Snider, Keith. 2018. Tone analysis for field linguists. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, C., Audrey Johnson P. Sharon, and Benita Gonzáles de Guzmán. 2013. Diccionario básico del mixteco de Xochapa, Guerrero. México: Instituto Lingüístico de Verano.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanton, Michael, and Juana Mendoza Ruiz. 2021. Observaciones sobre la diacronía del tono en el Tu’un Savi (Mixteco) de Alcozauca de Guerrero. In Estudios lingüísticos y filológicos en lenguas indígenas mexicanas. Celebración por los 30 años de seminario de lenguas Indígenas, eds. Francisco Arellanes and Lilián Guerrero. UNAM: Instituto de Investigaciones Filológicas (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Trubetzkoy, N. S. 1939/1969. Principles of phonology. Trans. by Christiane A.M. Baltaxe from Trubetzkoy, N.S. 1958. Grunzüge der Phonologie. Göttingen/Berkeley: Vandenhoeck and Ruprecht/University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zec, Draga. 1999. Footed tones and tonal feet: Rhythmic constituency in a pitch-accent language. Phonology 16(2): 225–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, Eva. 2016. Allomorphy between tone and segments: an Autosegmental account of Yucunany Mixtepec Mixtec. In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zylstra, Carol F. 1980. Phonology and morphophonemics of the Mixtec of Alacatlazala, Guerrero. SIL-Mexico Workpapers 4: 15–42.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Ryan Bennett and three anonymous reviewers and the editors of Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, especially the Associate Editor Michael Kenstowicz, for their invaluable comments and suggestions which improved the paper significantly. We would also like to acknowledge the speaker of Tu’un Savi (Mixtec) in the community of Alcozauca de Guerrero for their collaboration. This project has been funded by the Institute of Philological Research at the National Autonomous University of Mexico, and the project PAPIIT-IN404019, Complejidad paradigmática y tonal de las lenguas otomangues, National Autonomous University of Mexico, awarded to the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hiroto Uchihara.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Uchihara, H., Mendoza Ruiz, J. Minimality, maximality and perfect prosodic word in Alcozauca Mixtec. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 40, 599–649 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-021-09517-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-021-09517-y

Keywords

Navigation