Skip to main content
Log in

Locality in exceptions and derived environments in vowel harmony

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The regular realm of vowel harmony in Assamese consists of right-to-left regressive [Atr] harmony. In contrast with this regular pattern of vowel harmony, the exceptional Assamese processes dealt with in this paper are symptomatic of the behavior of a pair of morphemes that trigger additional processes not seen elsewhere in the language. This pair of morphemes allows raising of the otherwise opaque vowel // and fronting/backing of // depending on the [Back] quality of a mid vowel adjacent to //. Raising is strictly local in the presence of preceding high and low vowels, but there is also another pattern which shows backness assimilation to a previous vowel if there are mid vowels preceding the // of the input. This exceptional raising occurs to allow [Atr] harmony to spread regressively by changing the [−Atr] low vowel into a [+Atr] mid vowel. I analyse these cases within Optimality Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004) and show that these exceptional occurrences are morpheme-specific. It is also shown that these exceptional occurrences lend themselves to an account based on indexation of markedness constraints (Flack 2007; Ota 2004; Pater 2000, 2006, 2009). Consequently, the Assamese examples show that indexed markedness constraints are able to deal with an exceptional alternation where a low vowel undergoes harmony locally. This article also shows that an emergence of the unmarked analysis is required to account for the low back vowel that alternates with a front vowel if there is a preceding front vowel. This article goes beyond the problems encountered in Assamese, and claims that there is no need to invoke locality in exceptional blocking in vowel harmony, as both exceptional and non-exceptional blocking in vowel harmony are always local and bounded. The goal of this paper is to shed light on exceptional and emergent processes, arguing that they are always local and governed by strong universal properties of grammar.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Some combinations, like /ɔ/ + /ε/ were found not to exist in the native vocabulary.

  2. The other possibility is the inclusion of Alignment constraints, but Alignment in a regressive system does not provide us with an adequate explanation as there is no linguistically significant morphological or prosodic edge which can be used to designate edge alignment. It can also potentially result in candidates which are aligned to the wrong edge (Hansson 2001).

  3. Throughout this paper, the symbol is used to indicate the candidate which is the attested form, but fails to be the optimal candidate in the tableau. The symbol indicates the candidate which is not the attested form, but which is the winner as a result of the constraint hierarchy.

  4. The grounding conditions in Archangeli and Pulleyblank’s (1994) work pertain to the fact that tongue root advancement ([Atr]) and tongue body raising ([High]) are articulatorily synchronized, while tongue root advancement ([Atr]) is detrimental to the production of tongue body lowering ([Low]).

  5. This has been reported for [Atr] harmony in Hall and Hall (1980), mostly in West African languages (e.g. Wolof, Fula, Diola Fogny). In these systems, the organizing principle is such that [+Atr] vowels are dominant and [−Atr] vowels are recessive, so that opaque vowels can block the harmony propagated by the triggering [+Atr] vowel and start their own harmony domain.

  6. There is no evidence that the second vowel harmonizes in /CCVC/ sequences. As has been exhaustively discussed, [e] and [o] are allophonic in Assamese and should occur only in harmonic environments (i.e. in the presence of /i/ and /u/ to the right). In the absence of underlying /e/ and /o/, we cannot claim that the resulting [ε] and [ɔ] appear as a result of the preceding //.

  7. The origin of the suffix /-iy/ can be traced to Sanskrit /-i+k+/ and /-uw/ to the Sanskrit suffix /-u+k+/ which implies loveableness, affection, scornfulness, etc. of the object implicated. In some instances of Standard Colloquial Bengali /i/ contracts to /e/ and /u/ to /o/. (Both are counterparts of /-iy/ and /-uw/ respectively.) (Tunga 1995)

  8. See Bakovic (2000) for an analysis of ‘re-pairing’ in Maasai, Turkana and Turkish. In the specific case of Turkish, prospective [æ] are prohibited by re-pairing the mid [−Low −Back] with the [+Low +Back] vowel []. Although this is a characteristic of the entire phonology of Turkish, in Assamese ‘raising’ is the result of harmony triggered only by the exceptional morphemes.

  9. /-iy/ and /-uw/ behave in a largely equivalent manner as far as their involvement in exceptional patterns is concerned. Therefore, any reference to only one of them implies reference to the other one as well.

  10. Although in the literature on vowel harmony, it is not uncommon to find consonants which also block vowel harmony (see Archangeli and Pulleyblank 1994; Krämer 2003; Mahanta 2008; Woock and Noonan 1979).

  11. A reviewer suggests the option of positing a special faithfulness constraint Ident[Back]/[−Low] which specifies that back values of input [−Low] vowels should be preserved. Faithfulness constraints of any type, special or general, are not able to enforce front harmony in the data attested here. The constraint Ident[Back]/[−Low] would be ineffective in prohibiting the candidate without any [Back] harmony from emerging as the successful candidate, such that when the input is /εlh/+/uw/, both the unsuccessful candidate */elohuw/ and the actually attested form /elehuw/ will fare equally well with the constraint Ident[Back]/[−Low]. In the absence of any other markedness constraint which prohibits the // → [o] alternation, the evaluation will then select */elohuw/ because it will perform better with Ident[Back].

  12. The dictionary scanned for this purpose was Barua (1900/2007).

  13. Another word /xehotiy/ (also cited in Mahanta 2008) shows that [e…o] does not occur in the root because [o] is a part of the suffix: /xεh/+/ɔtiy/. These restrictions in the non-derived domain do not extend to morphologically complex words showing that such combinations of /ε…ɔ/ are highly productive.

  14. The markedness of rounded back vowels has been captured by Kaun (1995) by using the constraint *ROLO, a featural markedness constraint which prohibits vowels which are pronounced with a lower jaw position.

  15. Alternatively, the conjoined constraint could also be *[−Back][+Back]&Ident[Low]. The conjoining of this sequential markedness constraint with the faithfulness constraint would not lead to any improvement in the results predicted by LC.

References

  • Alderete, John. 2001. Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonology 18: 201–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alderete, John, Jill Beckman, Laura Benua, Amalia Gnanadesikan, John McCarthy, and Suzanne Urbanczyk. 1999. Reduplication with fixed segmentism. Linguistic Inquiry 30: 327–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen R. 1980. Problems and perspectives in the description of vowel harmony. In Issues in vowel harmony, ed. Robert M. Vagoo, 1–48. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anttila, Arto. 1997. Deriving variation from grammar. In Variation, change and phonological theory, eds. Frans Hinskens, Roeland van Hout, and Leo Wetzels, 35–68. Amsterdam: John Benjamin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anttila, Arto. 2002. Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 20(1): 1–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, Haruo. 1966. Nez Perce vowel harmony and proto-Sahaptian vowels. Language 42: 759–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archangeli, Diana, and Douglas Pulleyblank. 1994. Grounded phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakovic, Eric. 2000. Harmony, dominance and control. PhD diss., Rutgers University, New Brunswick.

  • Barua, Hem Chandra. 1900/2007. Hemkosha—The Assamese-English dictionary. Gauhati: Hemkosh Prakashan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benua, Laura. 1997. Transderivational identity: Phonological relations between words. PhD diss., Univ. of Massachusetts, Amherst.

  • Beckman, Jill. 1995. Shona height harmony: Markedness and positional identity. In University of Massachusetts occasional papers 18: Papers in optimality theory, eds. Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, and Suzanne Urbanczyk, 53–75. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, Jill. 1997. Positional faithfulness, positional neutralisation and Shona vowel harmony. Phonology 14: 1–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckman, Jill. 1998. Positional faithfulness. PhD diss., University of Massachusetts, Amherst Rutgers Optimality Archive 1297, http://roa.rutgers.edu/.

  • Benua, Laura. 2000. Phonological relations between words. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butska, Luba. 1998. Faithfulness to [voice] in Ukrainian: An analysis of voicing alternations within Optimality Theory. In RuLing Papers 1: Working papers from Rutgers University, eds. R. Artstein and M. Holler, 59–73. New Brunswick: Rutgers University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, Mi-Hui. 1994. Vowel harmony in Korean: A grounded phonology approach. PhD diss., Indiana University, Bloomington.

  • Chomsky, Noam, and Morris Halle. 1968. Sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crothers, John. 1978. Typology and universals of vowel systems. In Universals of human language. Vol 2: Phonology, eds. Joseph Harold Greenberg, Charles Albert Ferguson, and Edith Moravcsik, eds. 93–152. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimmendaal, Gert. 1983. The Turkana language. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flack, Kathryn. 2007. Templatic morphology and indexed markedness constraints. Linguistic Inquiry 38(4): 749–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finley, Sara. 2010. Exceptions in vowel harmony are local. Lingua 120: 1549–1566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fukuzawa, Haruka. 1999. Theoretical implications of OCP effects on features in optimality theory. PhD diss., University of Maryland, College Park.

  • Gouskova, Maria. 2007. The reduplicative template in Tonkawa. Phonology 24: 367–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, Beatrice L., R.M.R. Hall, and Martin D. Pam. 1974. African vowel harmony systems from the vantage point of Kalenjin. Afrika und Ubersee 57: 241–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, Beatrice L., and R.M.R. Hall. 1980. Nez Perce vowel harmony: An Africanist explanation and some theoretical questions. In Issues in vowel harmony: The Proceedings of the CUNY linguistics conference on vowel harmony, ed. Robert M. Vago, Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, James W. 1986a. Epenthesis processes in Spanish. In Studies in Romance languages, eds. Carol Neidle and Rafael A. Nunez-Cedeno, 107–122. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, James W. 1986b. El modelo multidimensional de la fonologia y dialectologia Caribena. In Estudios sobre la fonologia del Espanol del Caribe, eds. Rafael A. Nunez-Cedeno, Iraset Paez Urdaneta, and Jorge M. Guitart, 41–51. Caracas: La Casa de Bello.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansson, Gunnar. 2001. Theoretical and typological issues in consonant harmony. PhD diss., University of California, Berkeley.

  • Inkelas, Sharon. 1998. The theoretical status of morphologically conditioned phonology: A case study from dominance. In Yearbook of morphology 1997, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 121–155. Amsterdam: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inkelas, Sharon, and Cheryl Zoll. 2005. Reduplication: Doubling in morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Inkelas, Sharon, and Cheryl Zoll. 2003. Is grammar dependence real? Rutgers Optimality Archive 303, http://roa.rutgers.edu/.

  • Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 1999. The Phonological lexicon. In Handbook of Japanese linguistics, ed. Natsuko Tsujimura, 62–100. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2001. Covert generalizations in optimality theory: The role of stratal faithfulness constraints. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology 7(2): 273–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2003. On the sources of opacity in OT: Coda processes in German. In The syllable in optimality theory, eds. Caroline Fery and Rueben van de Vijver, 271–303. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kaun, Abigail. 1995. The typology of rounding harmony: An optimality theoretic approach. PhD diss., University of California, Los Angeles.

  • Kim, Jong-Kyoo. 2000. Quantity-sensitivity and feature-sensitivity of vowels: A constraint-based approach to Korean vowel phonology. PhD diss., Indiana University, Bloomington.

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1982. Explanation in phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1993. Blocking in non-derived environments. In Studies in lexical phonology, eds. Sharon Hargus and Ellen Kaisse, 277–314. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchner, Robert. 1996. Synchronic chain shifts in optimality theory. Linguistic Inquiry 27: 341–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krämer, Martin. 2003. Vowel harmony and correspondence theory. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kraska-Szlenk, Iwona. 1997. Exceptions in phonological theory. In Proceedings of the 16th international congress of linguists, Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kraska-Szlenk, Iwona. 1999. Syllable structure constraints in exceptions. In Phonologica 1996: Syllables!?, eds. John Rennison and Klaus Kühnhammer, 113–131. The Hague: Thesus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, Duck-Young. 1998. Korean phonology. Munich: Lincom Europa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardi, Linda. 1996. Positional faithfulness and voicing assimilation in optimality theory. Ms. University of Maryland, College Park.

  • Lombardi, Linda. 1999. Positional faithfulness and voicing assimilation in optimality theory. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 17(2): 267–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubowicz, Anna. 2002. Derived environment effects in optimality theory. Lingua 112: 243–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubowicz, Anna. 2003. Local conjunction and comparative markedness. Theoretical Linguistics 29: 101–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubowicz, Anna. 2005. Locality of conjunction. In Proceedings of the west coast conference on formal linguistics, Vol. 24, eds. John Alderete, Han Chung-hye, and Alexei Kochetov, 254–262. Cambridge: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahanta, Shakuntala. 2008. Locality and directionality in vowel harmony, Dissertation Series, Vol. 173, Utrecht: LOT.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John. 2003a. Comparative markedness. Theoretical Linguistics 29: 1–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John. 2003b. OT constraints are categorical. Phonology 20: 75–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John. 2005. Taking a free ride in morphophonemic learning. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 4: 19–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1986. Prosodic morphology. Report no. RuCCS-TR-32. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science.

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1994. Emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in prosodic morphology. In Proceedings of the Northeast linguistic society, ed. Gonzalez Merce, 333–379. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In University of Massachusetts occasional papers in linguistics 18: Papers in optimality theory, eds. Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, and Suzanne Urbanczyk, 249–384. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, John, and Matthew Wolf. 2005. Less than zero: Correspondence and the null output. Rutgers Optimality Archive 722, http://roa.rutgers.edu/.

  • Mohanan, Karuvannur P. 1986. The theory of lexical phonology. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreton, Elliott, and Paul Smolensky. 2002. Typological consequences of local constraint conjunction. In Proceedings of the west coast conference on formal linguistics, Vol. 21, eds. Line Mikkelsen and Christopher Potts, 306–319. Cambridge: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1996. Sign-based morphology and phonology: With special attention to optimality theory. PhD diss., University of California, Berkeley.

  • Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1998. Cyclic and noncyclic effects in a declarative grammar. In Yearbook of morphology 1997, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 179–218. Amsterdam: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1999. Sign-based morphology: A declarative theory of phonology-morphology interleaving. In The derivational residue in phonological optimality theory, eds. Ben Hermans and Marc van Oostendorp, 247–267. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orgun, Cemil Orhan, and Sharon Inkelas. 2002. Reconsidering bracket erasure. In Yearbook of morphology 2001, eds. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 115–146. Amsterdam: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ota, Mitsuhiko. 2004. The learnability of the stratified phonological lexicon. Journal of Japanese Linguistics 20: 19–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pater, Joe. 2000. Nonuniformity in English stress: The role of ranked and lexically specific constraints. Phonology 17: 237–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pater, Joe. 2006. The locus of exceptionality: Morpheme-specific phonology as constraint indexation. In University of Massachusetts occasional papers in linguistics 32: Papers in optimality theory III, eds. Leah Bateman, Michael O’Keefe, Ehren Reilly, and Adam Werle, 1–36. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pater, Joe. 2007. The locus of exceptionality: Morpheme-specific phonology as constraint indexation. In University of massachusetts occasional papers 32: Papers in optimality theory III, eds. Leah Bateman, Michael O’Keefe, Ehren Reilly, and Adam Werle, 259–296. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pater, Joe. 2009. Morpheme-specific phonology: Constraint indexation and inconsistency resolution. In Phonological argumentation: Essays on evidence and motivation, ed. Steve Parker, 123–154. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul M. 1968. Mohawk vowel doubling. International Journal of American Linguistics 35: 291–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. RuCCS Technical Report #1, Rutgers center for cognitive science, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, 1993. Revised version published 2004. Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Malden: Blackwell.

  • Riggle, Jason, and Colin Wilson. 2004. Local optionality. Poster presented at The north east linguistic society meeting 35, October, 2004.

  • Shademan, Shabnam. 2003. Epenthetic vowel harmony in Farsi. Paper presented at The twenty-second west coast conference on formal linguistics, University of California, San Diego, March, 2003.

  • Smolensky, Paul. 1995. On the structure of the constraint component Con of UG. Paper presented at University of California, Los Angeles. April, 1995.

  • Stewart, John M. 1967. A theory of the origin of Akan vowel harmony. In Proceedings of the sixth international congress of phonetic sciences 1967, eds. Bohuslav Hala, Milan Romportl, and Premysl Janot, 863–864. Prague: Academic publishing house of the Czech academy of sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, Donca. 1997. Phonetics in phonology: The case of laryngeal neutralization. Ms. University of California, Los Angeles. http://www.linguistics.ucla.edu/people/steriade/papers/phoneticsinphonology.pdf.

  • Stewart, John. 1971. Niger-Congo, Kwa. In Current trends in linguistics 7: Linguistics in sub-Saharan Africa, ed. Sebeok, Thomas A., 179–212. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunga, Sudhanshu. 1995. Bengali and other related dialects of South Assam. New Delhi: Mittal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woock, Edith B., and Michael Noonan. 1979. Vowel harmony in Lango. Proceedings of the Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society 15: 20–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, Colin. 2003. Analyzing unbounded spreading with constraints: Marks, targets and derivations. Ms. University of California, Los Angeles.

  • Wilson, Colin. 2006. Unbounded spreading is myopic. Paper presented at The workshop on current perspectives on phonology, Indiana University, Bloomington, June, 2006.

  • Zoll, Cheryl. 1997. Conflicting directionality. Phonology 14(2): 263–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This paper owes much of its existence to various people who have helped me in understanding the problem at hand, and primary among them is Joe Pater. I express my sincere gratitude to three anonymous reviewers for offering concrete suggestions to improve the paper. The associate editor Junko Itô helped in numerous ways with extensive comments and suggestions. This paper would not have been publishable without their valuable input. Another version of this paper was presented at the VIIth Asian GLOW colloquium in Hyderabad and I am grateful to the audience, especially Paul Kiparsky, for comments. Earlier versions of this work have benefitted from the comments of Wim Zonneveld, Janet Grijzenhout, and Kie Zuraw. All remaining errors are mine.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shakuntala Mahanta.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mahanta, S. Locality in exceptions and derived environments in vowel harmony. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 30, 1109–1146 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-012-9173-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-012-9173-5

Keywords

Navigation