Skip to main content
Log in

Reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Food neophobia is the fear of new foods, and it is associated with negative health outcomes. Recent work suggests that it is negatively correlated with the ability to discriminate visually similar dishes, but only in color. This led to a novel prediction: that colorblindness would be negatively associated with food neophobia. Here, we replicated that colorblind men (n = 103) are less food neophobic than non-colorblind men (n = 273), even when participants are unaware that being colorblind or not was relevant to their selection. We extend these results to food disgust, and find that both food neophobia and food disgust increase with age in colorblind men, whereas they are stable in noncolorblind men. These results underscore the role of color perception on affective attitudes towards food, in the absence of demand characteristics or manipulation of color in images. Our results advance our understanding of non-perceptual and affective consequences of colorblindness. They also have possible implications for improving treatment approaches in cases of severe food neophobia.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data are available on figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22674622.v1.

Notes

  1. Because Sun and Gauthier (2023) had not tested the effect of age in their study, we re-analyzed those results and found the age × color group interaction on agreeableness in that study (t = 2.101, p =  − .037). In that sample, agreeableness did not vary with age in non colorblind men (mean for young = 3.4, SD = .89; mean for old = 3.41, SD = .89) whereas it was lower for older colorblind men (mean for young = 3.83, SD = .71; mean for old = 3.57, SD = .88).

References

  • Ammann, J., Hartmann, C., & Siegrist, M. (2018). Does food disgust sensitivity influence eating behaviour? Experimental validation of the food disgust scale. Food Quality and Preference, 68, 411–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Annette, M., & Stafford, L. D. (2023). How colour influences taste perception in adult picky eaters. Food Quality and Preference, 105, 104763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, M., Risch, N., Hamburger, R., Mandel, B., Kushner, S., Newman, M., Drumer, D., & Belmaker, R. H. (1987). Genetic linkage between X-chromosome markers and bipolar affective illness. Nature, 326(6110), 289–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Birch, J. (2012). Worldwide prevalence of red-green color deficiency. JOSA A, 29(3), 313–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bratko, D., Butković, A., & Vukasović Hlupić, T. (2017). Heritability of personality. Psihologijske Teme, 26(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Callender, J. C., & Osburn, H. G. (1979). An empirical comparison of coefficient alpha, Guttman’s lambda-2, and MSPLIT maximized split-half reliability estimates. Journal of Educational Measurement, 1, 89–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castro, M., & Chambers, E. (2019). Willingness to eat an insect based product and impact on brand equity: A global perspective. Journal of Sensory Studies, 34(2), e12486. https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12486

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, S131–S133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denissen, J. J., Neumann, L., & Van Zalk, M. (2010). How the internet is changing the implementation of traditional research methods, people’s daily lives, and the way in which developmental scientists conduct research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34(6), 564–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). The Mini-IPIP scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality. Psychological Assessment, 18(2), 192–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovey, T. M. (2018). 16—Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder: An eating disorder on a spectrum with food neophobia. In S. Reilly (Ed.), Food neophobia (pp. 329–349). Woodhead Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Druschel, B. A., & Sherman, M. F. (1999). Disgust sensitivity as a function of the Big Five and gender. Personality and Individual Differences, 26(4), 739–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egolf, A., Siegrist, M., & Hartmann, C. (2018). How people’s food disgust sensitivity shapes their eating and food behaviour. Appetite, 127, 28–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foroni, F., Pergola, G., & Rumiati, R. I. (2016). Food color is in the eye of the beholder: The role of human trichromatic vision in food evaluation. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 37034.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon, L., Kupers, R., & Ptito, M. (2013). Reduced taste sensitivity in congenital blindness. Chemical Senses, 38(6), 509–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, I. (2022). Does food recognition depend on color? Figshare. Dataset. 10.6084/m9.figshare.20913772.v1

  • Gauthier, I. (2023). Data for reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men. 10.6084/m9.figshare.22674622.v1

  • Gauthier, I., & Fiestan, G. (2022). Food neophobia predicts visual ability in the recognition of prepared food. Food Quality and Preference, 103, 104702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. J. (1997). Becoming a “greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vision Research, 37(12), 1673–1682. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00286-6

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, C., & Siegrist, M. (2018). Development and validation of the food disgust scale. Food Quality and Preference, 63, 38–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazley, D., Stack, M., Walton, J., McNulty, B. A., & Kearney, J. M. (2022). Food neophobia across the life course: Pooling data from five national cross-sectional surveys in Ireland. Appetite, 171, 105941.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ishihara, S. (1918). Tests for color blindness. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 1(5), 376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jain, N., Wang, A., Henderson, M. M., Lin, R., Prince, J. S., Tarr, M. J., & Wehbe, L. (2023). Selectivity for food in human ventral visual cortex. Communications Biology, 6(1), 175.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • JASP Team. (2023). JASP (0.18.0) [Computer Software]. https://jasp-stat.org/

  • Kennedy, O. B., Stewart-Knox, B. J., Mitchell, P. C., & Thurnham, D. I. (2005). Flesh colour dominates consumer preference for chicken. Appetite, 44(2), 181–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Khosla, M., Murty, N. A. R., & Kanwisher, N. (2022). A highly selective response to food in human visual cortex revealed by hypothesis-free voxel decomposition. Current Biology, 32, 4159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knaapila, A., Tuorila, H., Silventoinen, K., Keskitalo, K., Kallela, M., Wessman, M., Peltonen, L., Cherkas, L. F., Spector, T. D., & Perola, M. (2007). Food neophobia shows heritable variation in humans. Physiology & Behavior, 91(5), 573–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lafraire, J., Rioux, C., Giboreau, A., & Picard, D. (2016). Food rejections in children: Cognitive and social/environmental factors involved in food neophobia and picky/fussy eating behavior. Appetite, 96, 347–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavin, T. A., & Hall, D. G. (2001). Domain effects in lexical development: Learning words for foods and toys. Cognitive Development, 16(4), 929–950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobos, P., & Januszewicz, A. (2019). Food neophobia in children. Pediatric Endocrinology Diabetes and Metabolism, 25(3), 150–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macario, J. F. (1991). Young children’s use of color in classification: Foods and canonically colored objects. Cognitive Development, 6(1), 17–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcontell, D. K., Laster, A. E., & Johnson, J. (2003). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of food neophobia in adults. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17(2), 243–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGugin, R. W., Tanaka, J. W., Lebrecht, S., Tarr, M. J., & Gauthier, I. (2011). Race-specific perceptual discrimination improvement following short individuation training with faces. Cognitive Science, 35(2), 330–347. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01148.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennock, I. M. L., Racey, C., Allen, E. J., Wu, Y., Naselaris, T., Kay, K., Franklin, A., & Bosten, J. (2022). Color-biased regions in the ventral visual pathway are food-selective. BioRxiv, 174, 393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pliner, P., & Hobden, K. (1992). Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite, 19(2), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/0195-6663(92)90014-W

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pliner, P., & Loewen, R. (2002). The effects of manipulated arousal on children’s willingness to taste novel foods. Physiology & Behavior, 76(4–5), 551–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rabadán, A., & Bernabéu, R. (2021). A systematic review of studies using the Food Neophobia Scale: Conclusions from thirty years of studies. Food Quality and Preference, 93, 104241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rioux, C., Lafraire, J., & Picard, D. (2018a). Visual exposure and categorization performance positively influence 3-to 6-year-old children’s willingness to taste unfamiliar vegetables. Appetite, 120, 32–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rioux, C., Leglaye, L., & Lafraire, J. (2018b). Inductive reasoning, food neophobia, and domain-specificity in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 47, 124–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Personality traits change in adulthood: Reply to Costa and McCrae (2006).

  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Disgust: Preadaptation and the cultural evolution of a food-based emotion. Food Preferences and Taste, 1, 65–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Roige, S., Gray, J. C., MacKillop, J., Chen, C.-H., & Palmer, A. A. (2018). The genetics of human personality. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 17(3), e12439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarin, H. V., Taba, N., Fischer, K., Esko, T., Kanerva, N., Moilanen, L., Saltevo, J., Joensuu, A., Borodulin, K., Männistö, S., Kristiansson, K., & Perola, M. (2019). Food neophobia associates with poorer dietary quality, metabolic risk factors, and increased disease outcome risk in population-based cohorts in a metabolomics study. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 110(1), 233–245. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqz100

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schickenberg, B., Van Assema, P., Brug, J., & De Vries, N. K. (2008). Are the Dutch acquainted with and willing to try healthful food products? The role of food neophobia. Public Health Nutrition, 11(5), 493–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegrist, M., & Hartmann, C. (2020). Perceived naturalness, disgust, trust and food neophobia as predictors of cultured meat acceptance in ten countries. Appetite, 155, 104814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegrist, M., Hartmann, C., & Keller, C. (2013). Antecedents of food neophobia and its association with eating behavior and food choices. Food Quality and Preference, 30(2), 293–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, J., & Gauthier, I. (2023). Does food recognition depend on color? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, M., Kimura, R., Kido, Y., Inoue, T., Moritani, T., & Nagai, N. (2017). Color of hot soup modulates postprandial satiety, thermal sensation, and body temperature in young women. Appetite, 114, 209–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thibodeau, M., Yang, Q., Ford, R., & Pickering, G. (2021). English version of the food disgust scale: Optimization and other considerations. Journal of Sensory Studies, 36(2), e12639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Heuvel, E., Newbury, A., & Appleton, K. M. (2019). The psychology of nutrition with advancing age: Focus on food neophobia. Nutrients, 11(1), 151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Van Eerdewegh, P. (1989). Linkage analysis with cohort effects: An application to X-linkage. Genetic Epidemiology, 6(1), 271–276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, K. P., Al-Shawaf, L., Lewis, D. M., & Wehbe, Y. S. (2023). Food neophobia and disgust, but not hunger, predict willingness to eat insect protein. Personality and Individual Differences, 202, 111944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, A.C.-N., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2009). Conditions for facelike expertise with objects: Becoming a ziggerin expert—But which type? Psychological Science, 20(9), 1108–1117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02430.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xie, J. Z., Tarczy-Hornoch, K., Lin, J., Cotter, S. A., Torres, M., Varma, R., Multi-Ethnic Pediatric Eye Disease Study Group. (2014). Color vision deficiency in preschool children: The multi-ethnic pediatric eye disease study. Ophthalmology, 121(7), 1469–1474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all our participants. The work was supported by the David K. Wilson Chair Research Fund from Vanderbilt University to Isabel Gauthier.

Funding

This work was supported by the David K. Wilson Chair Research Fund from Vanderbilt University to Isabel Gauthier.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Isabel Gauthier.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures in this study were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Vanderbilt Institute [IRB #222116] and conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and later amendments.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gauthier, I., Olatunji, B. Reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men. Motiv Emot 48, 198–208 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w

Keywords

Navigation