Skip to main content
Log in

Beliefs about the ability to control specific emotions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

People hold divergent beliefs regarding the controllability of their emotions. These beliefs can refer to emotion, in general, or to a particular emotion. But are beliefs about particular emotions distinct and emotion-specific or do they capture one general construct? To address this question, in this investigation, we tested the emotion-specificity of such beliefs. In Study 1 (N = 244), we assessed beliefs about the ability to control sadness, anger, and disgust, cross-sectionally. Beliefs about the ability to control specific emotions were associated but psychometrically distinct. As expected, beliefs about the ability to control a specific emotion were largely associated with experiences of that emotion at both the trait and state levels, although there was some overlap. In Study 2 (N = 157), we tested beliefs about the ability to control sadness and irritation in daily life, over 7 daily diaries. As expected, beliefs about the ability to control a specific emotion were associated with the respective trait emotion, and prospectively and differentially predicted experiences of that emotion in daily life. These findings demonstrate that although there is some commonality across them, beliefs about the ability to control particular emotions are emotion-specific. Accordingly, to better understand the experience and regulation of specific emotions, it may be useful to assess beliefs about the controllability of those emotions, in particular.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

All the data reported in the present manuscript, as well as the Supplemental Materials, are publicly available via Open Science Framework, and can be accessed now at: https://osf.io/4fk63/?view_only=8c387424ca4e460ba5aff88685721f99.

Notes

  1. All the data reported in the present manuscript, as well as the Supplemental Materials, are publicly available via Open Science Framework, and can be accessed at: https://osf.io/4fk63/?view_only=932b3ecd5299483cb9dbd58ba5dd9979.

  2. Data in all studies were collected as part of larger studies designed to answer multiple research questions (see descriptions in Supplemental Materials). We only report variables relevant to this project.

  3. As another proxy for trait sadness, we also measured neuroticism. Analyses with neuroticism are reported in the Supplementary Materials.

  4. We repeated these analyses using only the sadness item. Results remained unchanged, such that beliefs about the ability to control sadness were associated with state sadness (Estimate [SE] =  − 0.22 [0.08], 95% CI [− 0.37 to − 0.07], p = .004), and with trait sadness (Estimate [SE] =  − 0.27 [0.06], 95% CI [− 0.39 to − 0.16], p < .001).

  5. Due to the low reliability of the state disgust scale, we repeated this analysis using only the disgust item. Results remained unchanged, such that beliefs about the ability to control disgust were not associated with state disgust [Estimate (SE) =  − 0.08 [0.05], 95% CI [− 0.18 to 0.03], p = .151).

  6. The 9-day study included a pre-diary assessment, 7 days of diaries, and a post-diary assessment. Of the original 166 participants, 70.5% completed all 9 days, 7.8% completed 8 days, 6.6% completed 7 days, 5.4% completed 6 days, and 4.2% completed 5 days. Nine participants (5.4%) completed 4 days or less, and thus were excluded from the analyses. Participants completed on average 8.16 surveys (SD = 1.58).

  7. We repeated these analyses using only the sadness item. Results remained unchanged, such that beliefs about the ability to control sadness were associated with state sadness [Estimate (SE) =  − 7.41 (1.02), 95% CI (− 9.41 to − 5.42), p < .001], and with trait sadness [Estimate (SE) =  − 0.26 (0.09), 95% CI (− 0.44 to − 0.09), p = .004].

References

  • Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risktaking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: Recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(2), 128–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An introduction to diary and experience sampling research. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Castella, K., Goldin, P., Jazaieri, H., Ziv, M., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2013). Beliefs about emotion: Links to emotion regulation, well-being, and psychological distress. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(6), 497–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Castella, K., Goldin, P., Jazaieri, H., Ziv, M., Heimberg, R. G., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion beliefs in social anxiety disorder: Associations with stress, anxiety, and well-being. Australian Journal of Psychology, 66(2), 139–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, B. Q., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Why beliefs about emotion matter: An emotion-regulation perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 74–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, B. Q., Lwi, S. J., Gentzler, A. L., Hankin, B., & Mauss, I. B. (2018). The cost of believing emotions are uncontrollable: Youths’ beliefs about emotion predict emotion regulation and depressive symptoms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 47, 1170–1190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in everyday life. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, & J. N. Hugues (Eds.), Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 13–35). American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gutentag, T., Oliver, J., Gross, J. J., & Tamir, M. (2020). Incremental theories of emotion and intelligence across time: Temporal dynamics and correlates of change. Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000945

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1995). Multivariate data analyses with readings. Englewood Cliffs.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Pistorello, J., & Levin, M. E. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy as a unified model of behavior change. The Counseling Psychologist, 40(7), 976–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, J. S. (2015). Support for the domain specificity of implicit beliefs about persons, intelligence, and morality. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 195–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • IBM Corporation. (2012). IBM SPSS statistics for Windows, version 21.0. IBM Corp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kappes, A., & Schikowski, A. (2013). Implicit theories of emotion shape regulation of negative affect. Cognition & Emotion, 27(5), 952–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, R. B., & Dela-Rosa, E. D. (2019). Are your emotions under your control or not? Implicit theories of emotion predict well-being via cognitive reappraisal. Personality and Individual Differences, 138, 177–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kneeland, E. T., & Dovidio, J. F. (2019). Emotion malleability beliefs and coping with the college transition. Emotion, 20(3), 452–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kneeland, E. T., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Dovidio, J. F., & Gruber, J. (2016). Beliefs about emotion’s malleability influence state emotion regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 740–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Christensen, R. H. B., & Brockhoff, P. B. (2013). Different tests on lmer objects (of the lme4 package): Introducing the lmerTest package. In The R User Conference, useR! 2013 July 10–12 2013 University of Castilla-La Mancha, Albacete, Spain (Vol. 10, No. 30, p. 66). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13

  • Miron-Shatz, T., Stone, A., & Kahneman, D. (2009). Memories of yester-day’s emotions: Does the valence of experience affect the memory-experience gap? Emotion, 9(6), 885–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., & Elwood, L. S. (2007). The Disgust Scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 281–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, D. M., Meyvis, T., & Davidenko, N. (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 867–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romero, C., Master, A., Paunesku, D., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Academic and emotional functioning in middle school: The role of implicit theories. Emotion, 14(2), 227–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schleider, J. L., & Weisz, J. R. (2016). Implicit theories relate to youth psychopathology, but how? A longitudinal test of two predictive models. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 47(4), 603–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroder, H. S., Callahan, C. P., Gornik, A. E., & Moser, J. S. (2019a). The fixed mindset of anxiety predicts future distress: A longitudinal study. Behavior Therapy, 50(4), 710–717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schroder, H. S., Dawood, S., Yalch, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Moser, J. S. (2015). The role of implicit theories in mental health symptoms, emotion regulation, and hypothetical treatment choices in college students. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(2), 120–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schroder, H. S., Dawood, S., Yalch, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., & Moser, J. S. (2016). Evaluating the domain specificity of mental health–related mind-sets. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(6), 508–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroder, H. S., Kneeland, E. T., Silverman, A. L., Beard, C., & Björgvinsson, T. (2019b). Beliefs about the malleability of anxiety and general emotions and their relation to treatment outcomes in acute psychiatric treatment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(2), 312–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, K., Zaki, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2014). Addressing the empathy deficit: Beliefs about the malleability of empathy predict effortful responses when empathy is challenging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(3), 475–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S., & Crane, R. (1983). Assessment of anger: The State-Trait Anger Scale. In J. N. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 2, pp. 112–134). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, M. (2021). Effortful emotion regulation as a unique form of cybernetic control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1), 94–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, M., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Implicit theories of emotion: Affective and social outcomes across a major life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 731–744.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tullett, A. M., & Plaks, J. E. (2016). Testing the link between empathy and lay theories of happiness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(11), 1505–1521.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tongeren, D. R., & Burnette, J. L. (2018). Do you believe happiness can change? An investigation of the relationship between happiness mindsets, well-being, and satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 13(2), 101–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was supported by the Artery Chair in Personality Studies Endowed by Goldberg, Geller and Luria (M.T.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tony Gutentag.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gutentag, T., Kalokerinos, E.K. & Tamir, M. Beliefs about the ability to control specific emotions. Motiv Emot 47, 448–460 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-022-09991-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-022-09991-w

Keywords

Navigation