Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 38, Issue 6, pp 790–801 | Cite as

I don’t care about others’ approval: Dysphoric individuals show reduced effort mobilization for obtaining a social reward

  • Kerstin Brinkmann
  • Jessica Franzen
  • Cyrielle Rossier
  • Guido H. E. Gendolla
Original Paper

Abstract

Past research on reduced reward responsiveness in depression and dysphoria has mainly focused on monetary rewards. However, social rewards are important motivators and might be especially impaired in depression. The present study tested the hypothesis that nondysphoric individuals would mobilize more effort during a memory task without a clear performance standard when anticipating social approval for good performance. In contrast, dysphoric individuals were expected to be less sensitive to this reward and to mobilize less effort. Effort mobilization in this 2 (dysphoric vs. nondysphoric) × 2 (no reward vs. social approval) between-persons study was operationalized by participants’ cardiovascular reactivity. Results confirmed that nondysphorics had higher reactivity of systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and heart rate when expecting to enter their name in the alleged “best list”, whereas dysphorics had lower cardiovascular reactivity. The present study expands evidence for reduced reward responsiveness in depression and dysphoria from an effort mobilization perspective by demonstrating reduced effort-related cardiovascular reactivity to social rewards.

Keywords

Depression Effort mobilization Cardiovascular reactivity Social reward Reward responsiveness 

References

  1. Belani, K., Ozaki, M., Hynson, J., Hartmann, T., Reyford, H., Martino, J.-M., et al. (1999). A new noninvasive method to measure blood pressure. Anesthesiology, 91, 686–692. doi:10.1097/00000542-199909000-00021.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Cardiac psychophysiology and autonomic space in humans: Empirical perspectives and conceptual implications. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 296–322. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.296.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bijttebier, P., Beck, I., Claes, L., & Vandereycken, W. (2009). Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory as a framework for research on personality–psychopathology associations. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 421–430. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.04.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bonin, P., Méot, A., Aubert, L., Malardier, N., Niedenthal, P. M., & Capelle-Toczek, M.-C. (2003). Normes de concrétude, de valeur d’imagerie, de fréquence subjective et de valence émotionnelle pour 866 mots. L’Année Psychologique, 103, 655–694. doi:10.3406/psy.2003.29658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brehm, J. W., Wright, R. A., Solomon, S., Silka, L., & Greenberg, J. (1983). Perceived difficulty, energization, and the magnitude of goal valence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 21–48. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(83)90003-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bress, J. N., Foti, D., Kotov, R., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2013). Blunted neural response to rewards prospectively predicts depression in adolescent girls. Psychophysiology, 50, 74–81. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01485.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bress, J. N., Smith, E., Foti, D., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Neural response to reward and depressive symptoms in late childhood to early adolescence. Biological Psychology, 89, 156–162. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.004.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brinkmann, K., & Franzen, J. (2013). Not everyone’s heart contracts to reward: Insensitivity to varying levels of reward in dysphoria. Biological Psychology, 94, 263–271. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.07.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brinkmann, K., Schüpbach, L., Ancel Joye, I., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009). Anhedonia and effort mobilization in dysphoria: Reduced cardiovascular response to reward and punishment. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74, 250–258. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.009.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brownley, K. A., Hurwitz, B. E., & Schneiderman, N. (2000). Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 224–264). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Bruno, D., & Rutherford, A. (2010). How many response options? A study of remember–know testing procedures. Acta Psychologica, 134, 125–129. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chentsova-Dutton, Y., & Hanley, K. (2010). The effects of anhedonia and depression on hedonic responses. Psychiatry Research, 179, 176–180. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.013.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Costello, C. G. (1972). Depression: Loss of reinforcers or loss of reinforcer effectiveness? Behavior Therapy, 3, 240–247. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(72)80084-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dichter, G. S. (2010). Anhedonia in unipolar major depressive disorder: A review. The Open Psychiatry Journal, 4, 1–9. doi:10.2174/1874354401004010001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dickson, J. M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2006). Dysphoric adolescents’ causal explanations and expectancies for approach and avoidance goals. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 177–191. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.03.007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dimidjian, S., Martell, C. R., Addis, M. E., & Herman-Dunn, R. (2008). Behavioral activation for depression. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual (4th ed., pp. 328–364). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  18. Drakeford, J. L., Edelstyn, N. M. J., Oyebode, F., Srivastava, S., Calthorpe, W. R., & Mukherjee, T. (2010). Recollection deficiencies in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 175, 205–210. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.08.010.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Eshel, N., & Roiser, J. P. (2010). Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 118–124. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.027.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Forbes, E. E. (2009). Where’s the fun in that? Broadening the focus on reward function in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 199–200. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.001.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Research review: Altered reward function in adolescent depression: What, when and how? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 3–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02477.x.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Forbes, E. E., Hariri, A. R., Martin, S. L., Silk, J. S., Moyles, D. L., Fisher, P. M., et al. (2009). Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 64–73. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081336.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fuhrer, R., & Rouillon, F. (1989). La version française de l’échelle CES-D (Center for epidemiologic studies—depression scale): Description et traduction de l’échelle d’autoévaluation. European Psychiatry, 4(3), 163–166.Google Scholar
  25. Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Memory & Cognition, 16, 309–313. doi:10.3758/bf03197041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gendolla, G. H. E. (2012). Implicit affect primes effort: Theory and research on cardiovascular response. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 86, 123–135. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.05.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gendolla, G. H. E., Brinkmann, K., & Silvestrini, N. (2012a). Gloomy and lazy? On the impact of mood and depressive symptoms on effort-related cardiovascular response. In R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications (pp. 139–155). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002). The joint effect of informational mood impact and performance-contingent consequences on effort-related cardiovascular response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 271–283. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.271.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2006). Cardiovascular reactivity during performance under social observation: The moderating role of task difficulty. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62, 185–192. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.04.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Wright, R. A. (2009). Effort. In D. Sander & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Oxford companion to the affective sciences (pp. 134–135). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Gendolla, G. H. E., Wright, R. A., & Richter, M. (2012b). Effort intensity: Some insights from the cardiovascular system. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 420–438). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Gotlib, I. H., Hamilton, J. P., Cooney, R. E., Singh, M. K., Henry, M. L., & Joormann, J. (2010). Neural processing of reward and loss in girls at risk for major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 380–387. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.13.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Decreased responsiveness to reward in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 711–724. doi:10.1080/02699930050117684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hockey, G. R. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 73–93. doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(96)05223-4.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jamieson, J. (2004). Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with difference scores. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 52, 277–283. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.009.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M., & D’Argembeau, A. (2008). Identity recognition and happy and sad facial expression recall: Influence of depressive symptoms. Memory, 16, 364–373. doi:10.1080/09658210801935413.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kelsey, R. M. (2012). Beta-adrenergic cardiovascular reactivity and adaptation to stress: The cardiac pre-ejection period as an index of effort. In R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications (pp. 43–60). Washington, DC: APA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Knutson, B., Bhanji, J. P., Cooney, R. E., Atlas, L. Y., & Gotlib, I. H. (2008). Neural responses to monetary incentives in major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 686–692. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.023.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kunisato, Y., Okamoto, Y., Ueda, K., Onoda, K., Okada, G., Yoshimura, S., et al. (2012). Effects of depression on reward-based decision making and variability of action in probabilistic learning. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 1088–1094. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.05.007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Levick, J. R. (2003). An introduction to cardiovascular physiology (4th ed.). London, UK: Arnold.Google Scholar
  41. Liu, W.-H., Chan, R. C. K., Wang, L.-Z., Huang, J., Cheung, E. F. C., Gong, Q.-Y., et al. (2011). Deficits in sustaining reward responses in subsyndromal and syndromal major depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, 1045–1052. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.018.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., Ironson, G. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measure of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology, 28, 701–711. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01017.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  44. MacPhillamy, D. J., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). Depression as a function of levels of desired and obtained pleasure. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 651–657. doi:10.1037/h0037467.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02343.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. McCabe, C., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2009). Neural representation of reward in recovered depressed patients. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 205, 667–677. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1573-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Meehl, P. E. (1975). Hedonic capacity: Some conjectures. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 39, 295–307.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Nelson, B. D., McGowan, S. K., Sarapas, C., Robison-Andrew, E. J., Altman, S. E., Campbell, M. L., et al. (2013). Biomarkers of threat and reward sensitivity demonstrate unique associations with risk for psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 662–671. doi:10.1037/a0033982.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Obrist, P. A. (1976). The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction as it appears today. Psychophysiology, 13, 95–107. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb00081.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York, NY: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Papillo, J. F., & Shapiro, D. (1990). The cardiovascular system. In L. G. Tassinary & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, and inferential elements (pp. 456–512). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Pechtel, P., Dutra, S. J., Goetz, E. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Blunted reward responsiveness in remitted depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 1864–1869. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.08.011.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pizzagalli, D. A., Holmes, A. J., Dillon, D. G., Goetz, E. L., Birk, J. L., Bogdan, R., et al. (2009a). Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 702–710. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., & Fava, M. (2009b). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: Evidence from a probabilistic reward task. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 76–87. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1983). Determinants of reduction in intended effort as a strategy for coping with anticipated failure. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, 412–422. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(83)90069-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Ramponi, C., Barnard, P. J., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (2004). Recollection deficits in dysphoric mood: An effect of schematic models and executive mode? Memory, 12, 655–670. doi:10.1080/09658210344000189.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Ramponi, C., Murphy, F. C., Calder, A. J., & Barnard, P. J. (2010). Recognition memory for pictorial material in subclinical depression. Acta Psychologica, 135, 293–301. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.015.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Rhodewalt, F., & Fairfield, M. (1991). Claimed self-handicaps and the self-handicapper: The relation of reduction in intended effort to performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 402–417. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(91)90030-T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Richter, M. (2010). Pay attention to your manipulation checks! Reward impact on cardiac reactivity is moderated by task context. Biological Psychology, 84, 279–289. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.02.014.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Richter, M. (2013). A closer look into the multi-layer structure of motivational intensity theory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 1–12. doi:10.1111/spc3.12007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2006). Incentive effects on cardiovascular reactivity in active coping with unclear task difficulty. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 216–225. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.003.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Incentive value, unclear task difficulty, and cardiovascular reactivity in active coping. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 294–301. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.12.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009). The heart contracts to reward: Monetary incentives and pre-ejection period. Psychophysiology, 46, 451–457. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00795.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of variance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Scherhag, A., Kaden, J. J., Kentschke, E., Sueselbeck, T., & Borggrefe, M. (2005). Comparison of impedance cardiography and thermodilution-derived measurements of stroke volume and cardiac output at rest and during exercise testing. Cardiovascular Drugs and Therapy, 19, 141–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Shankman, S. A., Nelson, B. D., Sarapas, C., Robison-Andrew, E. J., Campbell, M. L., Altman, S. E., et al. (2013). A psychophysiological investigation of threat and reward sensitivity in individuals with panic disorder and/or major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 322–338. doi:10.1037/a0030747.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Sherdell, L., Waugh, C. E., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Anticipatory pleasure predicts motivation for reward in major depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 51–60. doi:10.1037/a0024945.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sigall, H., & Mills, J. (1998). Measures of independent variables and mediators are useful in social psychology experiments: But are they necessary? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 218–226. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0203_5.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Smoski, M. J., Felder, J. N., Bizzell, J., Green, S. R., Ernst, M., Lynch, T. R., et al. (2009). fMRI of alterations in reward selection, anticipation, and feedback in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 118, 69–78. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.034.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34–50. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Steele, J. D., Kumar, P., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2007). Blunted response to feedback information in depressive illness. Brain, 130, 2367–2374. doi:10.1093/brain/awm150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Treadway, M. T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: Lessons from translational neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 537–555. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Vrieze, E., Pizzagalli, D. A., Demyttenaere, K., Hompes, T., Sienaert, P., de Boer, P., et al. (2013). Reduced reward learning predicts outcome in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 73, 639–645. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.014.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  76. Wright, R. A. (2008). Refining the prediction of effort: Brehm’s distinction between potential motivation and motivational intensity. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 682–701. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00093.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wright, R. A., Dill, J. C., Geen, R. G., & Anderson, C. A. (1998). Social evaluation influence on cardiovascular response to a fixed behavioral challenge: Effects across a range of difficulty levels. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 277–285. doi:10.1007/bf02886377.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Wright, R. A., Killebrew, K., & Pimpalapure, D. (2002). Cardiovascular incentive effects where a challenge is unfixed: Demonstrations involving social evaluation, evaluator status, and monetary reward. Psychophysiology, 39, 188–197. doi:10.1017/S0048577202011137.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applications in social psychology. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 255–307). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  80. Wright, R. A., Tunstall, A. M., Williams, B. J., Goodwin, J. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (1995). Social evaluation and cardiovascular response: An active coping approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 530–543. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.530.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Zhang, W.-N., Chang, S.-H., Guo, L.-Y., Zhang, K.-L., & Wang, J. (2013). The neural correlates of reward-related processing in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal of Affective Disorders. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.039.

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kerstin Brinkmann
    • 1
  • Jessica Franzen
    • 1
  • Cyrielle Rossier
    • 1
  • Guido H. E. Gendolla
    • 1
  1. 1.Geneva Motivation Lab, FPSE, Department of PsychologyUniversity of GenevaGeneva 4Switzerland

Personalised recommendations