Skip to main content
Log in

Changes in materialism, changes in psychological well-being: Evidence from three longitudinal studies and an intervention experiment

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Few studies have examined how changes in materialism relate to changes in well-being; fewer have experimentally manipulated materialism to change well-being. Studies 1, 2, and 3 examined how changes in materialistic aspirations related to changes in well-being, using varying time frames (12 years, 2 years, and 6 months), samples (US young adults and Icelandic adults), and measures of materialism and well-being. Across all three studies, results supported the hypothesis that people’s well-being improves as they place relatively less importance on materialistic goals and values, whereas orienting toward materialistic goals relatively more is associated with decreases in well-being over time. Study 2 additionally demonstrated that this association was mediated by changes in psychological need satisfaction. A fourth, experimental study showed that highly materialistic US adolescents who received an intervention that decreased materialism also experienced increases in self-esteem over the next several months, relative to a control group. Thus, well-being changes as people change their relative focus on materialistic goals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Importantly, this meta-analysis reported that measures which assess the relative importance of materialistic values and goals have stronger associations with well-being than do measures that assess the absolute importance of materialistic values and goals. Given this finding, as well as the long theoretical insistence in both the value (Rokeach 1973; Schwartz 1992) and materialism (Kasser and Ryan 1993) literatures that measures of relative values are more appropriate than are measures of absolute values, we focus here on the former rather than the latter type of assessments.

  2. We do not include in this review the longitudinal study conducted by Nickerson et al. (2003), as it measured materialism at one point in time and well-being several years later. Such a design precludes conclusions about whether changes in materialism are associated with changes in well-being.

  3. Both Kasser et al. (1995) and Kasser and Ryan (1993, Study 3) used Aspiration Index ratings from this sample at 18 years of age to test other hypotheses.

  4. We used a parallel regression format to examine whether changes in the three other goals assessed at both ages 18 and 30 (i.e., affiliation, community feeling, and self-acceptance) were related to changes in psychopathology. None of the results approached significance.

  5. Sheldon et al. (2004) and Niemiec et al. (2009) used the same sample and some of the same measures as in Study 2. However, neither tested the current hypotheses with the exact same set of data. Specifically, Sheldon et al. used a measure of personal strivings to assess goals (rather than the AI), used Time 1 and Time 2 data from the longitudinal study (rather than Time 1 and Time 3 data), and did not test whether need satisfaction mediated the relation between goal content and well-being. Niemiec et al. used Time 2 and Time 3 data (rather than Time 1 and Time 3 data), assessed the attainment (rather than the importance) of life goals, and focused on a broad array of intrinsic and extrinsic goals (rather than the specific goal of financial success).

  6. As in Study 1, we again used a parallel regression format to examine whether changes in the six other goals assessed at T1 and T3 (i.e., affiliation, community feeling, self-acceptance, physical health, image, and popularity) were related to changes in SWB. Results showed that increasing the relative importance placed on community feeling aspirations was related to improvements in SWB (β = .30, p < .001), but no other results approached significance.

  7. Although the RMSEA index of .10 is rather high, Rigdon (1996, pp. 375–376) writes that, “…when sample size is low, RMSEA may suggest rejecting a model that otherwise would be accepted. (Arguably, the problem lies in the rule of thumb for interpreting RMSEA …).”

  8. We collected parallel measures of saving and spending attitudes (each of which were composed of responses to a Likert-type scale, the Aspiration Index, and the $100 windfall scenario); analyses found that the intervention did not affect either of these composites, suggesting it primarily worked to decrease materialism (i.e., spending attitudes).

References

  • Abramson, P. R., & Inglehart, R. (1995). Value change in global perspective. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Árnadóttir, Ö., Dittmar, H., Garðarsdóttir, R.B., & Bond, R. (2010). “We used to want to be entrepreneurs, now we just want to be good people”: Value shifts in materialism lead to changes in well-being. Unpublished manuscript, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK.

  • Banerjee, R., & Dittmar, H. (2008). Individual differences in children’s materialism: The role of peer relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 17–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2005). Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: Inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 40, 373–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, M. A., Wilkie, J. E. B., Kim, J. K., & Bodenhausen, G. B. (2012). Cuing consumerism: Situational materialism undermines personal and social well-being. Psychological Science, 23, 517–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: Trait aspects of living in the material world. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 265–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buijzen, M., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2005). Parental mediation of undesired advertising effects. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 49, 153–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burroughs, J. E., & Rindfleisch, A. (2002). Materialism and well-being: A conflicting values perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 348–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaplin, L. N., & John, D. R. (2007). Growing up in a material world: Age differences in materialism in children and adolescents. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 480–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christopher, A. N., Lasane, T. P., Troisi, J. D., & Park, L. E. (2007). Materialism, defensive and assertive self-presentational tactics, and life satisfaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 1145–1162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1996). Life values and adolescent mental health. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cozzolino, P. J., Staples, A. G., Meyers, L. S., & Samboceti, J. (2004). Greed, death, and values: From terror management to transcendence management theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 278–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, P. J., Bauer, D. J., & Willoughby, M. T. (2004). Testing main effects and interactions in latent curve analysis. Psychological Methods, 9, 220–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, P. J., Bauer, D. J., & Willoughby, M. T. (2006). Testing and probing interactions in hierarchical linear growth models. In C. S. Bergeman & S. M. Boker (Eds.), The Notre Dame series on quantitative methodology, Volume 1: Methodological issues in aging research (pp. 99–129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derogatis, L. R., Rickels, K., & Rock, A. F. (1976). The SCL-90 and the MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dittmar, H. (2008). Consumer culture, identity, and well-being: The search for the ‘good life’ and the ‘body perfect’. Hove & New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., & Kasser, T. (2013). A meta-analysis of the materialism literature. Brighton: Manuscript under review, University of Sussex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finkel, S. E. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data, Sage University Paper Series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

  • Gagné, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in pro-social behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garðarsdóttir, R. B., Aspinall, C., & Dittmar, H. (2009). It’s not the money, it’s the quest for a happier self: The role of happiness and success motives in the link between financial goal importance and subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 1100–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giddens, J. L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). Material values are largely in the family: A twin study of genetic and environmental contributions to materialism. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 428–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grouzet, F. M. E., Kasser, T., Ahuvia, A., Fernandez-Dols, J. M., Kim, Y., Lau, S., et al. (2005). The structure of goal contents across 15 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 800–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hellevik, O. (2003). Economy, values, and happiness in Norway. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 243–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herjanic, B., & Reich, W. (1982). Development of a structured psychiatric interview for children: Agreement between child and parent on individual symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 307–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mbl.is (ed). (2009). We need to make the society more secure: The new Prime Minister’s speech. http://mbl.is/mm/frettir/innlent/2009/02/04/thurfum_ad_thetta_oryggisnetid/.

  • Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2005). Frugality, generosity, and materialism in children and adolescents. In K. A. Moore & L. H. Lippman (Eds.), What do children need to flourish? Conceptualizing and measuring indicators of positive development (pp. 357–373). New York: Springer Science.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2009). Shifting values in response to climate change. In R. Engelman, M. Renner, & J. Sawin (Eds.), 2009 state of the world: Into a warming world (pp. 122–125). New York: W. W. Norton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2011a). Capitalism and autonomy. In V. I. Chirkov, R. M. Ryan, & K. M. Sheldon (Eds.), Human autonomy in cross-cultural context: Perspectives on the psychology of agency, freedom, and well-being (pp. 191–206). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2011b). Cultural values and the well-being of future generations: A cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 206–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. C. (2002). Materialistic values and well-being in business students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 137–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., Cohn, S., Kanner, A. D., & Ryan, R. M. (2007). Some costs of American corporate capitalism: A psychological exploration of value and goal conflicts. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 281–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Sheldon, K. M. (2004). Materialistic values: Their causes and consequences. In T. Kasser & A. D. Kanner (Eds.), Psychology and consumer culture: The struggle for a good life in a materialistic world (pp. 11–28). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Zax, M., & Sameroff, A. J. (1995). The relations of maternal and social environments to late adolescents’ materialistic and prosocial aspirations. Developmental Psychology, 31, 907–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y., Kasser, T., & Lee, H. (2003). Self-concept, aspirations, and well-being in South Korea and the United States. Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 277–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lykins, E. L. B., Segerstrom, S. C., Averill, A. J., Evans, D. R., & Kemeny, M. E. (2007). Goal shifts following reminders of mortality: Reconciling posttraumatic growth and terror management theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1088–1099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maio, G. R., Pakizeh, A., Cheung, W.-Y., & Rees, K. J. (2009). Changing, priming, and acting on values: Effects via motivational relations in a circular model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 699–715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martos, T., & Kopp, M. S. (2012). Life goals and well-being: Does financial status matter? Evidence from a representative Hungarian sample. Social Indicators Research, 105, 561–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mick, D. G. (1996). Are studies of dark side variables confounded by socially desirable responding? The case of materialism. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 106–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, C., Schwarz, N., Diener, E., & Kahneman, D. (2003). Zeroing in on the dark side of the American dream: A closer look at the negative consequences of the goal for financial success. Psychological Science, 14, 531–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The path taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and extrinsic aspirations in post-college life. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 291–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2008). Economic survey of Iceland 2008: Executive summary. Retrieved the 12th of September 2010 from http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/63/41/40157429.pdf.

  • Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). The temporal satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70, 340–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (1978). What I think and feel: A revised measure of children’s manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 271–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richins, M. (2004). The material values scale: Measurement properties and development of a short form. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 209–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for materialism and its measurement: Scale development and validation. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 303–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rigdon, E. E. (1996). CFI versus RMSEA: A comparison of two indexes for structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 3, 369–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ring, K. (1984). Heading toward Omega: In search of the meaning of the near-death experience. New York: Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., O’Donnell, M., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Goal and personality trait development in emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 541–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., & Robins, R. W. (2000). Broad dispositions, broad aspirations: The intersection of personality traits and major life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1284–1296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romero, E., Gomez-Fraguela, J. A., & Villar, P. (2011). Life aspirations, personality traits and subjective well-being in a Spanish sample. European Journal of Personality, 26, 45–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Chirkov, V. I., Little, T. D., Sheldon, K. M., Timoshina, E., & Deci, E. L. (1999). The American dream in Russia: Extrinsic aspirations and well-being in two cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1509–1524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., & Zax, M. (1982). Early development of children at risk for emotional disorder. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47, 7, Serial No. 199.

  • Schor, J. B. (2004). Born to buy: The commercialized child and the new consumer culture. New York: Scribner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Cultural and individual value correlates of capitalism: A comparative analysis. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 52–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharpe, J. P., & Ramanaiah, N. V. (1999). Materialism and the five-factor model of personality. Psychological Reports, 85, 327–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M. (2005). Positive value change during college: Normative trends and individual differences. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 209–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., Abad, N., Ferguson, Y., Gunz, A., Houser-Marko, L., Nichols, C. P., et al. (2010). Persistent pursuit of need-satisfying goals leads to increased happiness: A 6-month experimental longitudinal study. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 39–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Gunz, A. (2009). Psychological needs as basic motives, not just experiential requirements. Journal of Personality, 77, 1467–1492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2008). Psychological threat and extrinsic goal striving. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Niemiec, C. P. (2006). It’s not just the amount that counts: Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 331–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: It’s both what you do and why you do it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475–486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solberg, E. G., Diener, E., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why are materialists less satisfied? In T. Kasser & A. D. Kanner (Eds.), Psychology and consumer culture: The struggle for a good life in a materialistic world (pp. 29–48). Washington DC: APA.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Srivastava, A., Locke, E. A., & Bortol, K. M. (2001). Money and subjective well-being: It’s not the money, it’s the motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 959–971.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lacefield, K., & Schurtz, D. R. (2010). Birth cohort increases in psychopathology among young Americans, 1938–2007: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 145–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Visir.is. (2009). The debts of Iceland. Retreived the 6th of July 2009 from http://www.visir.is/article/20090714/VIDSKIPTI06/302475507.

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The 18-year-old data collection in Study 1 was supported by the W. T. Grant Foundation (Grant 88113087); the 30-year-old data collection was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (Grant 1 P50 MH-59396). Study 2 was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH-53385). Study 4 was supported by a grant from the Marjorie Weil and Marvin Edward Mitchell Foundation. The authors thank Clara Baldwin, Melvin Zax, and Jill Gray for their assistance with Study 1 and Cicely Robinson for her assistance with Study 4.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tim Kasser.

Additional information

After the first author, the remaining authors are listed in alphabetical order within the study to which they most contributed, in the order that the four studies are reported in this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kasser, T., Rosenblum, K.L., Sameroff, A.J. et al. Changes in materialism, changes in psychological well-being: Evidence from three longitudinal studies and an intervention experiment. Motiv Emot 38, 1–22 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9371-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9371-4

Keywords

Navigation