Abstract
The present study examines the influence of achievement goal states on working memory under varying executive load. Seventy-six undergraduate students were randomly assigned to either a mastery-approach condition (goal was to develop self-referential competence), a performance-approach condition (goal was to demonstrate normative competence) or a control condition (no goal assigned) prior to completing the N-Back working memory task. Analyses revealed achievement goal effects on working memory under high executive load (3-back) but not under the less demanding loads (1-back, 2-back). Under high load, pursuit of a performance-approach goal resulted in poorer working memory processing than pursuit of a mastery-approach goal or no-goal control. Findings are unlikely to be confounded by cognitive ability, working memory capacity or state-anxiety. Contributions to the motivation–cognition interface and suggestions for future research are discussed.
This is a preview of subscription content,
to check access.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224–237.
Baddeley, A. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist, 7, 85–97.
Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. III). New York: Academic Press.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Tro¨tschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027.
Barker, K. L., McInerney, D. M., & Dowson, M. (2002). Performance approach, performance avoidance and depth of information processing: A fresh look at relations between students academic motivation and cognition. Educational Psychology, 22, 571–589.
Bengtsson, S. L., Hakwan, L. C., & Passingham, R. E. (2009). Motivation to do well enhances responses to errors and self-monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 797–804.
Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Kaplan, A. (2005). Motivational influences on transfer of problem-solving strategies. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 1–22.
Brophy, J. (2005). Goal theorists should move on from performance goals. Educational Psychologist, 40, 167–176.
Butler, R. (1993). Effects of task—and ego—achievement goals on information seeking during task engagement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 18–31.
Chen, G. M., Gully, S. M., Whiteman, J., & Kilcullen, R. N. (2000). Examination of relationships among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual differences, and learning performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 835–847.
Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769–786.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the big five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880–896.
DiCintio, M. J., & Parkes, J. (1997, August). The influence of goal orientation on working memory capacity. In Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048.
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.
Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York: The Guilford Press.
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232.
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461–475.
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 × 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519.
Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 613–628.
Elliot, A. J., Shell, M. M., Henry, K., & Maier, M. (2005). Achievement goals, performance contingencies, and performance attainment: An experimental test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 630–640.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.
Escribe, C., & Huet, N. (2005). Knowledge accessibility, achievement goals, and memory strategy maintenance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 87–104.
Friedman, R. S., & Foerster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013.
Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19, 476–482.
Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215.
Gevins, A., & Cutillo, B. (1993). Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working-memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 87, 128–143.
Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: Development of a scale to measure social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 129–142.
Graham, S., & Golan, S. (1991). Motivational influences on cognition: Task involvement, ego involvement, and depth of information processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 187–194.
Gray, J. R. (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach–withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal two-back task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 436–452.
Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316–322.
Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Zimerman, S. (2009). Automatic and flexible: The case of non-conscious goal pursuit. Social Cognition, 27, 20–36.
Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2008). Effects of incentive on working memory capacity: Behavioral and pupillometric data. Psychophysiology, 45, 119–129.
Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70, 151–179.
Horvath, M., Scheu, C. R., & DeShon, R. P. (2001). Goal orientation: Integrating theory and measurement. In Paper presented at the 16th annual conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA.
Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). Task values, achievement goals, and interest: An integrative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 398–416.
Ikeda, M., Iwanaga, M., & Seiwa, H. (1996). Test anxiety and working memory system. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 1223–1231.
Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Miura, T. K., & Colflesh, G. J. H. (2007). Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: A question of construct validity. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 33, 615–622.
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 657–690.
Klein, K., & Fiss, W. H. (1999). The reliability and stability of the Turner and Engle working memory task. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 31, 429–432.
Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., Brown, K. G., Salas, E., Smith, E. A., & Nason, E. R. (2001). Effects of training goals and goal orientation traits on multi-dimensional training outcomes and performance adaptability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85, 1–31.
Linnenbrink, E., Ryan, A., & Pintrich, P. (1999). The role of goals and affect in working memory functioning. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 213–230.
MacLeod, C., & Donnellan, A. M. (1993). Individual differences in anxiety and the restriction of working memory capacity. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 163–173.
Maddox, T., & Markman, A. B. (2010). The motivation–cognition interface in learning and decision-making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 106–110.
Mangos, P. M., & Steele-Johnson, D. (2001). The role of subjective task complexity in goal orientation, self-efficacy, and performance relations. Human Performance, 41, 169–186.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 24, 167–202.
Murayama, Y., & Elliot, A. J. (2011). Achievement motivation and memory: Achievement goals differentially influence immediate and delayed remember–know recognition memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1339–1348.
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.
Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Süß, H.-M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence—their correlation and their relation: A comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 61–65.
Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). The n-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 46–59.
Parkes, J., Balliett, T., & DiCintio, M. J. (1998, April). The influence of goal orientation on working memory capacity: The task demands matter. In Roundtable presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.
Pintrich, P. R., & Schrauben, B. (1992). Students’ motivational beliefs and their cognitive engagement in classroom academic tasks. In D. Schunk & J. Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom: Causes and consequences (pp. 149–183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Fossati, P., Lehericy, S., Poline, J. B., Pillon, B., et al. (2002). The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in humans: an fMRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 99, 5669–5674.
Raven, J. C. (1990). Advanced progressive matrices. Oxford, England: Oxford Psychologists Press.
Revelle, W. (1993). Individual differences in personality and motivation: Non-cognitive determinants of cognitive performance. In A. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 346–373). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Varied forms of persistence: When free-choice behavior is not intrinsically motivated. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 185–205.
Savine, A. C., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Motivated cognitive control: Reward incentives modulate preparatory neural activity during task-switching. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 10294–10305.
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.
Steele-Johnson, D., Beauregard, R. S., Hoover, P. B., & Schmidt, A. M. (2000). Goal orientation and task demand effects on motivation, affect, and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 724–738.
Szatkowska, I., Bogorodzki, P., Wolak, T., Marchewka, A., & Szeszkowski, W. (2008). The effect of motivation on working memory: An fMRI and SEM study. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90, 475–478.
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154.
VandeWalle, D. (1997). Development and validation of a work domain goal orientation instrument. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57, 995–1015.
VandeWalle, D., Brown, S. P., Cron, W. L., & Slocum, J. W. (1999). The influence of goal orientation and self-regulation tactics on sales performance: A longitudinal field test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 249–259.
Ward, J. R., Rogers, D. A., Byrne, Z. S., & Masterson, S. S. (2004). State versus trait goal orientation: Is there truly a difference? In Presented as a poster at the 19th annual conference of the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology in Chicago, Illinois on April 2–4, 2004.
Wegge, J., Kleinbeck, U., & Schmidt, K.-H. (2001). Goal setting and performance in working memory and short-term-memory tasks. In M. Erez & U. Kleinbeck (Eds.), Work motivation in the context of a globalizing economy (pp. 49–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Weiner, B., & Walker, E. L. (1966). Motivational factors in short-term retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 190–193.
Winters, D., & Latham, G. P. (1996). The effect of learning versus outcome goals on a simple versus a complex task. Group and Organization Management, 21, 236–250.
Acknowledgments
The first author would like to acknowledge financial support from Goldsmiths, University of London (Student Bursary). The second author would like to acknowledge financial support from the Australian Research Council (Project ID: DP0984782).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Avery, R.E., Smillie, L.D. The impact of achievement goal states on working memory. Motiv Emot 37, 39–49 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9287-4
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9287-4