Skip to main content

Dysphorics can control depressive mood’s informational impact on effort mobilization

Abstract

Individuals’ level of depression has been shown to systematically determine their amount of effort-related cardiovascular reactivity (see Brinkmann and Gendolla in Motiv Emot, 31:71–82, 2007; J Pers Soc Psychol, 94:146–157, 2008). By means of a mood cue manipulation the present study aimed at providing a conclusive test whether this is due to the informational impact of depressed mood. After habituation, students with low versus high depression scores worked on a memory task under “do-your-best” instructions. Half of them received a cue before the task, suggesting that their current mood may have an impact during task performance. As expected, dysphoric participants showed higher systolic blood pressure reactivity during task performance than nondysphorics when no cue was given. This pattern was reversed in the cue condition, indicating that dysphorics effectively managed to reduce the depressive mood impact on their task demand appraisals and effort mobilization.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1

Notes

  1. Recently, cardiac pre-ejection period (PEP; i.e., the time interval in ms between the onset of ventricular depolarization and the opening of the aortic valve) has been assessed in the framework of motivational intensity theory (Brehm and Self 1989) as a reliable measure of myocardial contractility (e.g., Annis et al. 2001; Richter et al. 2008). Therefore, we also assessed and analyzed PEP reactivity by means of the same impedance cardiograph and in the same way as described, for instance, by Brinkmann et al. (2009). Due to a software upgrade the sampling rate was 1,000 Hz without down-sampling—contrary to previous studies. Unfortunately, due to contact problems with the patient cable of the impedance cardiograph, there were no data recordings for a quarter of the sample. Analyses based on the reduced sample revealed no baseline differences between the four cells, Fs < 2.31, ps > .13, overall M = 101.85, SD = 9.20. With respect to PEP reactivity during task performance, there were no significant main or interaction effects, Fs < 3.02, ps > .09. Cell means and standard errors of PEP reactivity were as follows: Dysphoric-no cue M = −.01, SE = .66; nondysphoric-no cue M = −2.22, SE = 1.07; dysphoric-cue M = −3.59, SE = 1.63; nondysphoric-cue M = −2.31, SE = .64.

  2. Subjective frequency of the selected words ranged from 2.16 to 3.52 and subjective emotional valence ranged from 2.56 to 3.64 on the 5-point rating scales.

  3. These results hold also true when considering the overall mood scores, composed of the positive and reversed-coded negative adjectives (Cronbach’s αs > .76): There was a dysphoria main effect, F(1, 50) = 33.27, p < .001, η2 = .38, and a cue main effect, F(1, 50) = 4.56, p < .04, η2 = .05, before task performance and a dysphoria main effect, F(1, 50) = 17.03, p < .001, η2 = .25, after task performance. All positive, negative, and overall mood scores were correlated with the depression score, r(54)s > \( \left| {.43} \right| \), ps < .001. These correlations replicate previous findings when the CES-D scale was administered in the end, rather than at the beginning of the experimental session (see Brinkmann et al. 2009).

References

  • Abele, A. E., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (1999). Satisfaction judgments in positive and negative moods: Effects of concurrent assimilation and contrast producing processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 883–895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abele, A. E., & Petzold, P. (1994). How does mood operate in an impression formation task? An information integration approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Annis, S., Wright, R. A., & Williams, B. J. (2001). Interactional influence of ability perception and task demand on cardiovascular response: Appetitive effects at three levels of challenge. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 6, 82–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression—causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., Jaffee, S., Jo, E., & Troccoli, B. T. (2000). On the correction of feeling-induced judgmental biases. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking (pp. 131–152). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohner, G., & Weinerth, T. (2001). Negative affect can increase or decrease message scrutiny: The affect interpretation hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1417–1428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonin, P., Méot, A., Aubert, L., Malardier, N., Niedenthal, P. M., & Capelle-Toczek, M. -C. (2003). Normes de concrétude, de valeur d’imagerie, de fréquence subjective et de valence émotionnelle pour 866 mots. L’Année Psychologique, 103, 655–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, K., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Dysphoria and mobilization of mental effort: Effects on cardiovascular reactivity. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 71–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, K., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Does depression interfere with effort mobilization? Effects of dysphoria and task difficulty on cardiovascular response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 146–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, K., Schüpbach, L., Ancel Joye, I., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009). Anhedonia and effort mobilization in dysphoria: Reduced cardiovascular response to reward and punishment. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74, 250–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuhrer, R., & Rouillon, F. (1989). La version française de l’échelle CES-D (Center for epidemiologic studies-depression scale): Description et traduction de l’échelle d’autoévaluation. European Psychiatry, 4, 163–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (1998). The persistent use of negative affect by anxious individuals to estimate risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1350–1363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2000). Do you have to pay attention to your feelings to be influenced by them? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 698–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Abele, A. E., & Krüsken, J. (2001). The informational impact of mood on effort mobilization: A study of cardiovascular and electrodermal responses. Emotion, 1, 12–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Brinkmann, K. (2005). The role of mood states in self-regulation: Effects on action preferences and resource mobilization. European Psychologist, 10, 187–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002a). Informational mood impact on effort-related cardiovascular response: The diagnostic value of mood counts. Emotion, 2, 251–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002b). Mood, task demand, and effort-related cardiovascular response. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 577–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., Gilboa, E., & Sommerfeld, B. K. (2000). Cognitive functioning in depression: Nature and origins. In R. J. Davidson (Ed.), Anxiety, depression and emotion (pp. 133–163). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (2000). The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in memory. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 39, pp. 47–71). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., Levine, G. M., McDonald, H. E., Melton, J., & Martin, L. L. (1997). The role of mood in quantitative and qualitative aspects of performance: Single or multiple mechanisms? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 602–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G. R. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 73–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Siemer, M. (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and dysphoria: Difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 179–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 437–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kavanagh, D. J., & Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and sadness on perceived capabilities. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 507–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, M.-S., Kim, J.-J., & Kwon, J. S. (2001). The effect of immediate and delayed word repetition on event-related potential in a continuous recognition task. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 387–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Llabre, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P. G., Ironson, G. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measure of cardiovascular reactivity to behavioral challenges. Psychophysiology, 28, 701–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 166–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Rafaeli, E., & Yovel, I. (2003). Cognitive biases in emotional disorders: Information processing and social-cognitive perspectives. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 976–1009). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 29–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Poon, L. W., & Fozard, J. L. (1980). Age and word frequency effects in continuous recognition memory. Journal of Gerontology, 35, 77–86.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Hamilton, J. C., Herring, F. H., & Greenberg, J. (1989). Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 351–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richter, M., Friedrich, A., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Task difficulty effects on cardiac reactivity. Psychophysiology, 45, 869–875.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, W. D., & Cervone, D. (2002). The impact of negative affect on performance standards: Evidence for an affect-as-information mechanism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 19–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvestrini, N., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009). Mood-regulative hedonic incentive interacts with mood and task difficulty to determine effort-related cardiovascular response and facial EMG. Biological Psychology, 82, 54–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stapel, D. A., & Suls, J. (2007). Assimilation and contrast in social psychology. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tillema, J. L., Cervone, D., & Scott, W. D. (2001). Negative mood, perceived self-efficacy, and personal standards in dysphoria: The effects of contextual cues on self-defeating patterns of cognition. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 535–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1997). The flexible correction model: The role of naive theories of bias in bias correction. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 141–208). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, H. K., Tuomilehto, J., Kuulasmaa, K., Domarkiene, S., Cepaitis, Z., Molarius, A., et al. (1997). Blood pressure levels in the 41 populations of the WHO MONICA project. Journal of Human Hypertension, 11, 733–742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A. (2008). Refining the prediction of effort: Brehm’s distinction between potential motivation and motivational intensity. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 682–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applications in social psychology. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 255–307). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J. C., & Mischel, W. (1982). Influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 901–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kerstin Brinkmann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brinkmann, K., Grept, J. & Gendolla, G.H.E. Dysphorics can control depressive mood’s informational impact on effort mobilization. Motiv Emot 36, 232–241 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9236-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9236-7

Keywords

  • Depression
  • Dysphoria
  • Informational mood impact
  • Cardiovascular reactivity
  • Effort mobilization