Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 35, Issue 1, pp 14–22 | Cite as

Cognitive reappraisal increases heart rate variability in response to an anger provocation

  • Thomas F. Denson
  • Jessica R. Grisham
  • Michelle L. Moulds
Original Paper

Abstract

Adaptive self-regulatory responses to negative events are associated with good mental health, social functioning, and physical health. Two forms of emotion regulation that have received attention within the context of anger are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Research suggests that greater heart rate variability (HRV) is a physiological indicator of adaptive emotion regulation and decreased mental load. In the present experiment, we recorded HRV while 131 undergraduate women viewed an anger-inducing video of a fellow student arguing for a position counter to that of the participant on an important political issue. Immediately prior to viewing, participants were instructed to reappraise, suppress their emotions, or simply watch the video as normal. Participants in the reappraisal condition showed increased HRV whereas those in the suppression and control condition showed no such increase. These results provide support for increased HRV as a biological correlate of adaptive emotion regulation. One implication is that cognitive reappraisal might afford greater autonomic flexibility when an individual is confronted with anger-inducing events.

Keywords

Anger Reappraisal Suppression Heart rate variability Emotion regulation 

References

  1. Aasman, J., Mulder, G., & Mulder, L. J. (1987). Operator effort and the measurement of heart rate variability. Human Factors, 29, 161–170.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Appelhans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10, 229–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Balogh, S., Fitzpatrick, D. F., Hendricks, S. E., & Paige, S. R. (1993). Increases in heart rate variability with successful treatment in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychopharmacological Bulletin, 29, 201–206.Google Scholar
  4. Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 183–214.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berntson, G. G., Lozano, D. L., & Chen, Y.-J. (2005). Filter properties of root mean square successive difference (RMSSD) for heart rate. Psychophysiology, 42, 246–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Butler, E. A., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction. Psychophysiology, 43, 612–622.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Epstein, E. M., Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2005). Getting to the heart of the matter: Written disclosure, gender, and heart rate. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 413–419.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Regulatory control and adults’ stress-related responses to daily life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1107–1117.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Friedman, H. S., & Booth-Kewley, S. (1987). The ‘‘disease prone personality”: A meta-analytic view of the construct. American Psychologist, 42, 539–555.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Goedhart, A. D., van der Sluis, S., Houtveen, J. H., Willemsen, G., & de Geus, E. J. C. (2007). Comparison of time and frequency domain measures of RSA in ambulatory recordings. Psychophysiology, 44, 203–215.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Goodie, J. L., Larkin, K. T., & Schauss, S. (2000). Validation of the polar heart rate monitor for assessing heart rate during physical and mental stress. Journal of Psychophysiology, 14, 159–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 214–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hadase, M., Azuma, A., Zen, K., Asada, S., Kawasaki, T., Kamitani, T., et al. (2004). Very low frequency power of heart rate variability is a powerful predictor of clinical prognosis in patients with congestive heart failure. Circulation Journal, 68, 343–347.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal influence on working memory and attention. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 48, 263–274.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hosie, J. A., Milne, A. B., & McArthur, L. (2005). The after-effects of regulating anger and anger-related emotions on self-report ratings and behavior: Divergent consequences for men and women. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 48, 288–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Jönsson, P., & Sonnby-Borgström, M. (2003). The effects of pictures of emotional faces on tonic and phasic autonomic cardiac control in women and men. Biological Psychology, 62, 157–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kubzansky, L. D., Davidson, K. W., & Rozanski, A. (2005). The clinical impact of negative psychological states: Expanding the spectrum of risk for coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, S10–S14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. La Rovere, M. T., Pinna, G. D., Maestri, R., Mortara, A., Capomolla, S., Febo, O., et al. (2003). Short-term heart rate variability strongly predicts sudden cardiac death in chronic heart failure patients. Circulation, 107, 565–570.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lane, R. D., McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Chen, K., Ahern, G. L., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage, 44, 213–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Leon, I., Hernandez, J. A., Rodriguez, S., & Vila, J. (2009). When head is tempered by heart: Heart rate variability modulates perception of other-blame reducing anger. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Liao, D., Carnethon, M., Evans, G. W., Cascio, W. E., & Heiss, G. (2002). Lower heart rate variability is associated with the development of coronary heart disease in patients with diabetes—the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study. Diabetes, 51, 3524–3531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mauss, I. B., Cook, C. L., Cheng, J. Y. J., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal: Experiential and physiological responses to an anger provocation. International Journal of Psychopathology, 66, 116–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Memedovic, S., Grisham, J. R., Denson, T. F., & Moulds, M. L. (2010). The effects of trait reappraisal and suppression on anger and blood pressure in response to provocation. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 540–543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mendes, W. B. (2009). Assessing autonomic nervous system activity. In E. Harmon-Jones & J. S. Beer (Eds.), Methods in social neuroscience. New York, NY, USA: Guilford.Google Scholar
  26. Nowlis, V. (1965). Research with the mood adjective checklist. In S. S. Tompkins & C. S. Izard (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and personality: Empirical studies. Oxford, UK: Springer.Google Scholar
  27. Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion regulation: Insights from social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 153–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Penttilä, J., Helminen, A., Jartti, T., Kuusela, T., Huikuri, H. V., Tulppo, M. P., et al. (2001). Time domain, geometrical and frequency domain analysis of cardiac vagal outflow: Effects of various respiratory patterns. Clinical Physiology, 21, 365–376.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74, 116–143.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Quartana, P. J., & Burns, J. W. (2010). Emotion suppression affects cardiovascular responses to initial and subsequent laboratory stressors. British Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 511–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ray, D. R., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the mind’s eye? Anger rumination and reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 133–145.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 410–424.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sloan, D. M., & Epstein, E. M. (2005). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia predicts written disclosure outcome. Psychophysiology, 42, 611–615.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Stein, P. K., & Kleiger, R. E. (1999). Insights from the study of heart rate variability. Annual Review of Medicine, 50, 249–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stemmler, G. (1997). Selective activation of traits: Boundary conditions of the activation of anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 213–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Suls, J., & Wan, C. K. (1993). “The relationship between trait hostility and cardiovascular reactivity: A quantitative review and analysis. Psychophysiology, 30, 615–626.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Swain, A., & Suls, J. (1996). Reproducibility of blood pressure and heart rate reactivity: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology, 33, 162–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tattersall, A. J., & Hockey, G. R. (1995). Level of operator control and changes in heart rate variability during simulated flight maintenance. Human Factors, 37, 682–698.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 201–216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart–brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 81–88.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. van der Meij, L., Buunk, A. P., Almela, M., & Salvador, A. (2010). Testosterone responses to competition: The opponent’s psychological state makes it challenging. Biological Psychology, 84, 330–335.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Wranik, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). Why do I get angry? A componential appraisal approach. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger: Constituent and concomitant biological, psychological, and social processes (pp. 243–266). New York, NY, USA: Springer.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas F. Denson
    • 1
  • Jessica R. Grisham
    • 1
  • Michelle L. Moulds
    • 1
  1. 1.School of PsychologyThe University of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations