Skip to main content

When internalization leads to automatization: The role of self-determination in automatic stereotype suppression and implicit prejudice regulation

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that self-determined prejudice regulation is negatively related to both self-reported prejudice and automatic racial bias. However, the social-cognitive processes involved in this association have not yet been examined. Thus, the current project sought to test the ‘internalization-automatization hypothesis’, that is, to assess the extent to which prejudice regulation is automatic for those high and low in self-determined motivation to regulate prejudice. To this end, two different experimental paradigms were used. In Experiment 1 (N = 84), differences in the automatic activation and application of stereotypes were assessed for those high and low in self-determined prejudice regulation. As expected, both types of prejudice regulators showed similar stereotype activation. However, only self-determined individuals inhibited the application of stereotypes following a prime. Experiment 2 (N = 134), assessed the impact of self-regulatory depletion on the regulation of implicit prejudice. As anticipated, for the self-determined regulators, prejudice regulation did not vary between depleted and non-depleted individuals. However, when non-self-determined prejudice regulators were depleted, prejudice increased, relative to non-depleted controls. Results are discussed in terms of an increased understanding of prejudice regulation through self-determination. Evidence of the automatization of self-determined prejudice regulation offers promising implications for the reduction of prejudice.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1

Notes

  1. The Race IAT, used in the current research, measures implicit race bias by assessing people’s tendency to associate positive evaluations with White people and negative evaluations with Black people, and vice versa. Caucasians tend to categorize stereotype congruent concepts (e.g. White-Good) more quickly than stereotype-incongruent concepts (e.g. Black-Good). What is particularly relevant for the issue of depletion is that the IAT contains both an automatic and a controlled component. The automatic association is made in the stereotype-congruent pairing task, while controlled processes are required to override the dominant (i.e., stereotyped or prejudiced) response on the stereotype-inconsistent pairing task. Because respondents are required to make non-stereotypical responses, cognitive resources and control are said to be required. Presumably, performance on the stereotype-inconsistent task, for which self-regulation is required, will deteriorate when individuals are depleted, resulting in longer response latencies and greater automatic racial bias. Thus, to the extent that prejudice regulation taxes regulatory strength, self-regulation will fall short when it is depleted.

  2. The text used in the depletion manipulation was a page from a statistics textbook, whose content was unrelated to the goals and parameters of the present study.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 93–132). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against the controllability of automatic stereotype effects. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 361–382). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. The American Psychologist, 54, 462–479. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Ego depletion and self-control failure: An energy model of the self’s executive function. Self and Identity, 1, 129–136. doi:10.1080/152988602317319302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1253–1265. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bostic, T. J., Rubio, D. M., & Hood, M. (2000). A validation of the Subjective Vitality Scale using structural equation modeling. Social Indicators Research, 52(3), 313–324. doi:10.1023/A:1007136110218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., D’Alessandro, D. U., & Koestner, R. (2006). The differential effects of intrinsic and identified motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective, experimental, and implicit approaches to self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4), 750–762. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioural confirmation processes: The self- fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541–560. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit motivator: On the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Handbook of self-determination theory research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(5), 835–848. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.835.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(3), 316–326. doi:10.1177/0146167297233009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Hilden, L. E. (2001). Emotional reactions to a seemingly prejudiced response: The role of automatically activated racial attitudes and motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 538–549. doi:10.1177/0146167201275003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., & Hixon, G. (1991). The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 509–517. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.4.509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008). Implicit motivation to control prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 164–172. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). Automaticity in goal pursuit. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 624–646). New York: Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordijn, E. H., Hindriks, I., Koomen, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Consequences of stereotype suppression and internal suppression motivation: A self-regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 212–224. doi:10.1177/0146167203259935.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Govorun, O., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Ego-depletion and prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled components. Social Cognition, 24(2), 111–136. doi:10.1521/soco.2006.24.2.111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2005). The new unconscious. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects on training in the negation of stereotype associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klonis, S. C., Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2005). Internal and external motivation to respond without sexism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1237–1249. doi:10.1177/0146167205275304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z., & Spencer, S. J. (2003). When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 522–544. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., Grant, P., & Chung, J. (2007). On the self-regulation of implicit and explicit prejudice: A self-determination perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(5), 732–749. doi:10.1177/0146167206298564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levesque, C., & Pelletier, L. G. (2003). On the investigation of primed and chronic autonomous and heteronomous motivational orientations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(12), 1570–1584. doi:10.1177/0146167203256877.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., & Gashke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta-experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 102–111. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.1.102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 1024–1036. doi:10.1177/0146167206288008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monteith, M. J., Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Czopp, A. M. (2002). Putting the brakes on prejudice: On the development and operation of cues for control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1029–1050. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P. M., Wasel, W., & Schaal, B. (1999). Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egalitarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(1), 167–184. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M. (2008). Prejudice as self-control failure. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(2), 314–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., Gagné, M., & Rosman, H. (2008). Helpful self-control: Autonomy support, vitality, and depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 573–585. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 894–906. doi:10.1177/0146167203029007008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 774–789. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 811–832. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, J. J., Lassiter, G. D., Markman, K. D., & Snyder, C. J. (2006). Gender differences in attitudes toward gay men and lesbians: The role of motivation to respond without prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1325–1338. doi:10.1177/0146167206290213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richeson, J. A., & Trawalter, S. (2005). Why do interracial interactions impair executive function? A resource depletion account. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 934–947. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749–761. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). From ego depletion to vitality: Theory and findings concerning the facilitation of energy available to the self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(2), 702–717. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00098.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, L., & Kunda, Z. (1999). Reactions to a Black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicted stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 885–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1660–1672. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stucke, T. S., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Ego depletion and aggressive behavior: Is the inhibition of aggression a limited resource? European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(1), 1–13. doi:10.1002/ejsp.285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vohs, K. D., & Faber, R. J. (2007). Spent resources: self-regulatory resource availability affects impulse buying. The Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 537–547. doi:10.1086/510228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, S. C., Brinol, P., & Hermann, A. D. (2006). Resistance to persuasion as self-regulation: Ego-depletion and its effects on attitude change processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 150–156. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a doctoral fellowship awarded to the first author from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, and a research grant awarded to the second author from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa Legault.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Legault, L., Green-Demers, I. & Eadie, A.L. When internalization leads to automatization: The role of self-determination in automatic stereotype suppression and implicit prejudice regulation. Motiv Emot 33, 10–24 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9110-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9110-4

Keywords

  • Self-determination
  • Automaticity
  • Implicit motivation
  • Prejudice regulation
  • Stereotype
  • Suppression
  • Self-regulation