Abstract
Two experiments were conducted to investigate emotional associative learning to odors and subsequent behavioral effects. In Experiment 1, participants experienced a frustration mood induction in the presence of an unfamiliar ambient odor and later worked on puzzle tests in a room scented with either the same-odor, a different-odor, or no-odor. Participants in the same-odor condition spent significantly less time working on the tests than participants in the other conditions; however, test accuracy did not vary. To clarify the findings, Experiment 2 included a test-only control and an emotionally neutral same-odor conditions. Results were compatible with the conclusion that decreased time spent by participants in the negative-same-odor condition was due to emotions elicited by associative learning to the ambient odor, although alternative interpretations remain possible. These data extend our previous results with children and suggest that odors readily become associated to emotions and can thereby influence behavior.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
References
Alaoui-Ismaili, O., Robin, O., Rada, H., Dittmar, A., & Vernet-Maury, E. (1997). Basic emotions evoked by odorants: Comparison between autonomic responses and self-evaluation. Physiology & Behavior, 62(4), 713–720.
Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Schicker, I., Laska, M., Hudson, R., Distel, H., Kobayakawa, T., & Saito, S. (1998). Differences in perception of everyday odors: A Japanese-German cross-cultural study. Chemical Senses, 23(1), 31–38.
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., van den Bergh, O., & Crombez, G. (1990). Flavor–flavor and color–flavor conditioning in humans. Learning and Motivation, 21, 434–455.
Baeyens, F., & Wrzesniewksi, A., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1996). Toilet rooms, body massages, and smells: Two field studies on human evaluative odor conditioning. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 15, 77–96.
Balogh, R. D., & Porter, R. H. (1986). Olfactory preferences resulting from mere exposure in human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(4), 395–401.
Dalton, P., & Wysocki, C. J. (1996). The nature and duration of adaptation following long-term odor exposure. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(5), 781–792.
Davis, L. B., & Porter, R. H. (1991). Persistent effects of early odor exposure on human neonates. Chemical Senses, 16(2), 169–174.
Dilks, D., Dalton, P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1999). Cross-cultural variation in responses to malodors. Chemical Senses, 24, 599.
Ehrlichman, H., & Bastone, L. (1992). The use of odour in the study of emotion. In S. van Toller, & G. H. Dodd (Eds.), Fragrance—The psychology and biology of perfume (pp. 143–159). London: Elsevier Applied Science.
Ehrlichman, H., Kuhl, S. B., Zhu, J., & Warrenburg, S. (1997). Startle reflex modulation by pleasant and unpleasant odors in a between-subjects design. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 726–729.
Engen, T. (1988). The acquisition of odour hedonics. In S. van Toller, & G. H. Dodd (Eds.), Perfumery—The psychology and biology of fragrance (pp. 79–90). London: Chapman and Hall Publishers.
Engen, T. (1991). Odor sensation and memory. New York: Praeger.
Epple, G., & Herz, R. S. (1999). Ambient odors associated to failure influence cognitive performance in children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35(2), 103–107.
Feather, N. T. (1961). The relationship of persistence at a task to expectation of success and achievement related motives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), 552–561.
Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: Affective influences on interpersonal behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 1–28.
Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408.
Herz, R. S. (1997a). The effects of cue distinctiveness on odor-based context-dependent memory. Memory & Cognition, 25(3), 375–380.
Herz, R. S. (1997b). Emotion experienced during encoding enhances odor retrieval cue effectiveness. American Journal of Psychology, 110(4), 489–505.
Herz, R. S. (1999). Caffeine effects on mood and memory. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 37(9), 869–879.
Herz, R. S. (2001). Ah, sweet skunk: Why we like or dislike what we smell. Cerebrum, 3(4), 31–47.
Herz, R. S., Beland, S. L., & Hellerstein, M. (2004). Changing odor hedonic perception through emotional associations in humans. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 17, 315–339.
Hirt, E. R., Levine, G. M., McDonald, H. E., Melton, J., & Martin, L. L. (1997). The role of mood in quantitative and qualitative aspect of performance: Single or multiple mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 602–629.
Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 179–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.). Handbook of Emotions, 2nd Edition (pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford.
Isen, A., Rosenzweig, A. S., & Young, M. J. (1991). The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making, 11, 221–227.
Kavanaugh, J. K. (1987). The content of implicit leadership theories: An investigation of achievement orientation, task condition and relationship orientation under varied task difficulty and achievement conditions. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47(9-A), 3438.
Kirk-Smith, M. D., Van Toller, C., & Dodd, G. H. (1983). Unconscious odour conditioning in human subjects. Biological Psychology, 17(2–3), 221–231.
Knasko, S. C. (1992). Ambient odor’s effect on creativity, mood, and perceived health. Chemical Senses, 17(1), 27–35.
Knasko, S. C. (1995). Pleasant odors and congruency: Effects on approach behavior. Chemical Senses, 20(5), 479–487.
Lovibond, P. F., Davis, N. R., & O’Flaherty, A. S. (2000). Protection from extinction in human fear conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 967–983.
Lott, I., Sullivan, M., & McPherson, D. (1989). Associative olfactory learning occurs in the neonate. Neurology, 39, 110.
Martin, I., & Levey, A. B. (1978). Evaluative conditioning. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1, 57–102.
Martin, L. L., Achee, J. W., Ward, D. W., & Harlow, T. F. (1993). The role of cognition and effort in the use of emotions to guide behavior. In R. S. Wyer, & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 6, pp. 147–157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mennella, J. A. (1995). Mother’s milk: A medium for early flavor experiences. Journal of Human Lactation, 11(1), 39–45.
Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1991). The transfer of alcohol to human milk. Effects on flavor and the infant’s behavior. New England Journal of Medicine, 325(14), 981–985.
Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1993). The effects of repeated exposure to garlic-flavored milk on the nursling’s behavior. Pediatrics Research, 34(6), 805–808.
Miltner, W., Matjak, M., Braun, C., Diekmann, H., & Brody, S. (1994). Emotional qualities of odors and their influence on the startle reflex in humans. Psychophysiology, 31(1), 107–110.
Otto, T., Cousens, G., & Herzog, C. (2000). Behavioral and neuropsychological foundations of olfactory fear conditioning. Behavioral Brain Research, 110, 119–128.
Robin, O., Alaoui-Ismaili, O., Dittmar, A., & Vernet-Mauri, E. (1998). Emotional responses evoked by dental odors: An evaluation from autonomic parameters. Journal of Dental Research, 77, 1638–1946.
Rotton, J. (1983). Affective and cognitive consequences of malodorous pollution. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 4(2), 171–191.
Russ, S. W. (1993). Affect and creativity: The role of affect and play in the creative process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 3–30.
Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. (1989). Affect Grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.
Schiffman, S. S., Sattely-Miller, E. A., Suggs, M. S., & Graham, B. G. (1995). The effect of pleasant odors and hormone status on mood of women at midlife. Brain Research Bulletin, 36(1), 19–29.
Schiffman, S. S., Suggs, M. S., & Sattely-Miller, E. A. (1995). Effect of pleasant odors on mood of males at midlife: Comparison of African-American and European-American men. Brain Research Bulletin, 36(1), 31–37.
Schleidt, M., Hold, B., & Attili, G. (1981). A cross-cultural study on the attitude towards personal odors. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 7, 19–31.
Sullivan, M. J., & Conway, M. (1989). Negative affect leads to low-effort cognition: Attributional processing for observed social behavior. Social Cognition, 7(4), 315–337.
Sullivan, R. M., & Toubas, P. (1998). Clinical usefulness of maternal odor in newborns: Soothing and feeding preparatory responses. Biology of the Neonate, 74, 402–408.
Vermetten, E., & Bremner, J. D. (2003). Olfaction as a traumatic reminder in posttraumatic stress disorder: Case reports and review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(2), 202–207.
Wasserman, E. A., & Miller, R. R. (1997). What’s elementary about associative learning? Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 573–607.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Herz, R.S., Schankler, C. & Beland, S. Olfaction, Emotion and Associative Learning: Effects on Motivated Behavior. Motiv Emot 28, 363–383 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-004-2389-x
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-004-2389-x