Advertisement

Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy

, Volume 17, Issue 3, pp 397–402 | Cite as

Are physicians’ estimations of future events value-impregnated? Cross-sectional study of double intentions when providing treatment that shortens a dying patient’s life

  • Anders Rydvall
  • Niklas Juth
  • Mikael Sandlund
  • Niels LynøeEmail author
Scientific Contribution

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to corroborate or undermine a previously presented conjecture that physicians’ estimations of others’ opinions are influenced by their own opinions. We used questionnaire based cross-sectional design and described a situation where an imminently dying patient was provided with alleviating drugs which also shortened life and, additionally, were intended to do so. We asked what would happen to physicians’ own trust if they took the action described, and also what the physician estimated would happen to the general publics’ trust in health services. Decrease of trust was used as surrogate for an undesirable action. The results are presented as proportions with a 95 % Confidence Interval (CI). Statistical analysis was based on inter-rater agreement (Weighted Kappa)-test as well as χ 2 test and Odds Ratio with 95 % CI. We found a moderate inter-rater agreement (Kappa = 0.552) between what would happen with the physicians’ own trust in healthcare and their estimations of what would happen with the general population’s trust. We identified a significant difference between being pro et contra the treatment with double intentions and the estimation of the general population’s trust (χ2 = 72, df = 2 and p < 0.001). Focusing on either decreasing or increasing own trust and being pro or contra the action we identified a strong association [OR 79 (CI 25–253)]. Although the inter-rater agreement in the present study was somewhat weaker compared to a study about the explicit use of the term ‘physicians assisted suicide’ we found that our hypothesis—physicians’ estimations of others’ opinions are influenced by their own opinions—was corroborated. This might have implications in research as well as in clinical decision-making. We suggest that Merton’s ideal of disinterestedness should be highlighted.

Keywords

Value-based medicine Evidence-based medicine Disinterestedness Decision-making Personal values 

References

  1. Johansson, I., and N. Lynøe. 2008. Medicine and philosophy: A twenty-first century introduction. Frankfurt: Ontos Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Schulz, K.F., and D.A. Grimes. 2007. Blinding in randomized trials: Hiding who got what. Lancet 359: 696–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Lexchin, J., L.A. Bero, B. Djulbegovic, et al. 2003. Pharmaceutical industry sponsorship and research outcome and quality: Systematic review. BMJ 326: 1167–1170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Lexchin, J. 2011. Those who have the gold make the evidence: How the pharmaceutical industry biases the outcomes of clinical trials of medications. Sci Eng Ethics. Feb 15. [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
  5. Ricci, Z., and S. Romagnoli. 2011. Is disclosing conflicts of interest like purifying in the Ganges river? Journal of Critical Care 26(4): 429–430. Epub 2011 Jun 28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Juth, N., and N. Lynöe. 2010. Do strong value-based attitudes influence estimations of future events? Journal of Medical Ethics 36(4): 255–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Thulesius, H., H. Scott, G. Helgesson, et al. 2013. De-tabooing dying control: A grounded theory study. BMC Palliative Care 12: 13. doi: 10.1186/1472-684X-12-13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Rydvall, A., N. Juth, M. Sandlund, et al. 2013. To treat or not to treat a newborn child with a comprehensive brain-damage? A cross-sectional study of physicians’ and the general population’s perceptions of intentions. Med Health Care Philos Jun 16. [Epub ahead of print].Google Scholar
  9. Lindblad, A., R. Löfmark, and N. Lynöe. 2009. Would physician-assisted suicide jeopardize trust in medical services? An empirical study of attitudes among the general public in Sweden. Scand J Public Health 37: 260–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Helgesson, G., A. Lindblad, H. Thulesius, et al. 2009. Reasoning about physician assisted suicide: analysis of comments by physicians and the Swedish general public. Clin Ethics 4: 19–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Lynøe, N., M. Leijonhufvud. 2013. Police on an intensive-care unit: What can happen? J Med Ethics. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2012-100681. [Epub ahead of print].
  12. Haidt, J. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol Review 108: 814–834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Greene, J.D., L.E. Nystrom, A.D. Engell, et al. 2004. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgement. Neuron 44: 389–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clore, G.L., and J.R. Huntsinger. 2007. How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 393–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bosshard, G., S. Fischer, and A. van der Heide. 2006. Intentionally hastening death by withholding or withdrawing treatment. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift 118: 322–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lynøe, N. 2013. Physicians’ practices when frustrating patients’ need: A comparative study of restrictiveness offering abortion and sedation therapy. J Med Ethics 0: 1–4. doi: 10.1136/medethics-2012-101194.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anders Rydvall
    • 1
  • Niklas Juth
    • 2
  • Mikael Sandlund
    • 3
  • Niels Lynøe
    • 2
    Email author
  1. 1.Unit of Anesthesiology and Intensive Care Medicine, Department of Surgical and Perioperative SciencesUmeå UniversityUmeåSweden
  2. 2.Centre for Healthcare EthicsKarolinska InstitutetStockholmSweden
  3. 3.Unit of Psychiatry, Department of Clinical SciencesUmeå UniversityUmeåSweden

Personalised recommendations