Skip to main content
Log in

Malleability of taste perception: biasing effects of rating scale format on taste recognition, product evaluation, and willingness to pay

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Product-related cues, such as brand or price, can influence consumers’ taste perception. Going beyond this observation, we examine the extent to which a stimulus-extrinsic factor, such as the format of the measurement tool on which consumers describe attributes of a taste sample, influences concurrent taste perception, and in turn, later taste recognition, overall product evaluation, and willingness to pay (WTP). The results of two experiments show that rating scale format (i) influences consumers’ concurrent impression of a taste sample, (ii) systematically biases later identification of the sample in a taste recognition test, and (iii) affects overall product evaluation and WTP. However, scale format (iv) does not influence ratings and downstream judgments when consumers are highly knowledgeable in the product domain. These findings demonstrate that the experience of taste is fleeting and not well represented in memory, and that like other subjective experiences, taste needs to be reconstructed based on accessible cues.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We acknowledge that because crispness is related to acidity, it’s not obvious that it is necessarily positive.

References

  • Alba, J. W., & Hutchinson, J. W. (1987). Dimensions of consumer expertise. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(4), 411–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoshuk, L. M. (2000). Comparing sensory experiences across individuals: recent psychophysical advances illuminate genetic variation in taste perception. Chemical Senses, 25(4), 447–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradburn, N. M., Rips, L. J., & Shevell, S. K. (1987). Answering autobiographical questions: the impact of memory and inference on surveys. Science, 236(4798), 157–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, K. A. (1999). Postexperience advertising effects on consumer memory. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(4), 319–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, W. S. (1979). To know with the nose: keys to odor identification. Science, 2(4379), 467–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engen, T., & Ross, B. M. (1973). Long-term memory of odors with and without verbal descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100(2), 221–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eves, A. (1994). Sensory analysis—an alternative to wine tasting? International Journal of Wine Marketing, 6(2), 32–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garber, L., Hyatt, E., & Starr, R. (2000). The effects of food color on perceived flavor. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 8(4), 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawel, R. (1997). The use of language by trained and untrained experienced wine tasters. Journal of Sensory Studies, 12(4), 267–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, R. W., Ratner, R. K., & Thompson, D. V. (2011). Outpacing others: when consumers value products based on relative usage frequency. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 1079–1094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoegg, J., & Alba, J. W. (2007). Taste perception: more than meets the tongue. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4), 490–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughson, A. L., & Boakes, R. A. (2001). Perceptual and cognitive aspects of wine tasting expertise. Australian Journal of Psychology, 53(2), 103–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaegar, S. (2005). Consumer evaluation of novel kiwifruit: willingness-to-pay. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture, 85(15), 2519–2526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köster, E. P. (2003). The psychology of food choice: some often encountered fallacies. Food Quality and Preference, 14(5–6), 359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krishna, A. (2010). Sensory marketing: research on the sensuality of consumers. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawless, H. T. (1984). Flavor description of white wine by “expert” and nonexpert wine consumers. Journal of Food Science, 49(1), 120–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L., Frederick, S., & Ariely, D. (2006). Try it, you’ll like it: the influence of expectation, consumption, and revelation on preferences for beer. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1054–1058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockshin, L. S., & Rhodus, W. S. (1993). The effect of price and oak flavor on perceived wine quality. International Journal of Wine Marketing, 5(2–3), 13–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mantonakis, A., Rodero, P., Lesschaeve, I., & Hastie, R. (2009). Order in choice: effects of serial position on preferences. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1309–1312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melcher, J., & Schooler, J. W. (1996). The misremembrance of wines past: Verbal and perceptual expertise mediate verbal overshadowing effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(2), 231–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, G., Raghubir, P., & Schwarz, N. (1995). Behavioral frequency judgments: an accessibility-diagnosticity framework. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 212–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parr, W. V., Heatherbell, D., & White, G. K. (2002). Demystifying wine expertise: olfactory threshold, perceptual skill and semantic memory in expert and novice wine judges. Chemical Senses, 27(8), 747–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions modulate the neural representation of experienced pleasantness. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(3), 1050–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psychological Review, 96(2), 341–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., & Clore, J. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self-report: evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 198–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (1996). Cognition and communication: judgmental biases, research methods, and the logic of conversation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports: how the questions shape the answers. American Psychologist, 54(2), 93–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in context. Social Cognition, 25, 638–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Scheuring, B. (1988). Judgments of relationship satisfaction: inter- and intraindividual comparison strategies as a function of questionnaire structure. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 485–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Sudman, S. (1994). Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Hippler, H. J., Deutsch, B., & Strack, F. (1985). Response scales: effects of category range on reported behavior and subsequent judgments. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49, 388–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Strack, F., Müller, G., & Chassein, B. (1988). The range of response alternatives may determine the meaning of the question: further evidence on informative functions of response alternatives. Social Cognition, 6, 107–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Bohner, G., Harlacher, U., & Kellenbenz, M. (1991a). Response scales as frames of reference: the impact of frequency range on diagnostic judgment. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 37–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Knäuper, B., Hippler, H., Noelle-Neumann, E., & Clark, L. (1991b). Rating scales: numeric values may change the meaning of scale labels. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(4), 570–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., Kahneman, D., & Xu, J. (2009). Global and episodic reports of hedonic experience. In R. Belli, D. Alwin, & F. Stafford (Eds.), Using calendar and diary methods in life events research (pp. 157–174). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegrist, M., & Cousin, M. E. (2009). Expectations influence sensory experience in a wine tasting. Appetite, 52(3), 762–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, G. E. (1990). Psychology of novice and expert wine talk. American Journal of Psychology, 103(4), 495–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antonia Mantonakis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mantonakis, A., Schwarz, N., Wudarzewski, A. et al. Malleability of taste perception: biasing effects of rating scale format on taste recognition, product evaluation, and willingness to pay. Mark Lett 28, 293–303 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-016-9416-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-016-9416-z

Keywords

Navigation