Advertisement

Journal of Youth and Adolescence

, Volume 48, Issue 5, pp 1009–1021 | Cite as

Developing Morality, Competence, and Sociability in Adolescence: A Longitudinal Study of Gender Differences

  • Elisabetta CrocettiEmail author
  • Silvia Moscatelli
  • Goda Kaniušonytė
  • Wim Meeus
  • Rita Žukauskienė
  • Monica Rubini
Empirical Research

Abstract

Morality, competence, and sociability have been conceptualized as fundamental dimensions of social judgment that individuals use to evaluate themselves and other people and groups. The way in which adolescents perceive themselves along these dimensions affects the quality of their relationships across multiple social contexts. Given the centrality of morality, competence, and sociability for adolescents’ social life, the purpose of this study was to understand how these dimensions develop over time with a focus on gender differences, since males and females can show distinct trajectories due to socialization and developmental processes. Participants were 916 (51.4% girls; Mage = 15.64 years) adolescents involved in a three-wave longitudinal study with annual assessments. The findings highlighted that females reported increasing levels of morality and competence, while males showed decreasing levels in all dimensions. Furthermore, females also showed greater consistency in the configuration of morality, competence, and sociability, and inter-individual differences appeared to be already well-settled in each dimension for both males and females. Overall, this study increases the developmental understanding of how core dimensions of social judgment change in the adolescent phase, highlighting gender differences and similarities.

Keywords

Morality Competence Sociability Gender Development 

Notes

Authors’ Contributions

E.C., S.M., and M.R. conceived the current study; E.C. performed the statistical analyses. E.C., S.M., and M.R. wrote the manuscript; all authors (E.C., S.M., G.K., W.M., R.Z., M.R.) participated in the interpretation of the results and in the drafting of the article; R.Z. is the principal investigator of the POSIDEV project and is responsible for the data collection. All authors read and approved the final paper.

Funding

Data of the POSIDEV study were used for this study. POSIDEV was funded by the European Social Fund under the Global Grant measure, VP1-3.1-SMM-07-02-008 assigned to Rita Žukauskienė. Silvia Moscatelli and Monica Rubini received support for working on this article by a grant from the Italian Ministry of Research and Education, University and Research FIRB2012 (Protocollo RBFR128CR6_004) assigned to Silvia Moscatelli.

Data Sharing and Declaration

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Ethics Committee of the Mykolas Romeris University, Vilnius (Lithuania) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants (and from their parents, if minors) included in the study.

References

  1. Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. J Pers Soc Psychol, 93, 751–763.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition: A dual perspective model. In M. P. Zanna & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology 50, (195–255). Burlington: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  3. Alfieri, T., Ruble, D. N., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Gender stereotypes during adolescence: Developmental changes and the transition to junior high school. Dev Psychol, 32, 1129–1137.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.32.6.1129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Allison, S. T., Messick, D. M., & Goethals, G. R. (1989). On being better but not smarter than others: The Muhammad Ali effect. Soc Cogn, 7, 275–295.  https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1989.7.3.275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barth, J. M., Kim, H., Eno, C. A., & Guadagno, R. E. (2017). Matching abilities to careers for others and self: Do gender stereotypes matter to students in advanced math and science classes? Sex Roles, 79, 83–97.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-017-0857-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bem, S. L. (1998). An unconventional family. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Beunen, G., Thomis, M., Maes, H. H., Loos, R., Malina, R. M., & Claessens, A. L., et al. (2000). Genetic variance of adolescent growth in stature. Annals of Human Biol, 27, 173–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA: Bancroft Books.Google Scholar
  10. Boiché, J., Plaza, M., Chalabaev, A., Guillet-Descas, E., & Sarrazin, P. (2014). Social antecedents and consequences of gender-sport stereotypes during adolescence. Psychol Women Quarterly, 38, 259–271.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684313505844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., & Esposito, G. (2017). Continuity and stability in development. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 113–119.  https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12221.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive role of morality in social judgment. Soc Cogn, 32, 397–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Menegatti, M., & Moscatelli, S. (2016). Honesty and dishonesty don’t move together: trait content information influences behavioral synchrony. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 171–186.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0229-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Byrne, B. M. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. 2nd edn. New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
  15. Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cole, D. A., Maxwell, S. E., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. G., Seroczynski, A. D., Tram, J. M., & Maschman, T. (2001). The development of multiple domains of child and adolescent self-concept: a cohort sequential longitudinal design. Child Dev, 72, 1723–1746.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00375.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Colom, R., & Lynn, R. (2004). Testing the developmental theory of sex differences in intelligence on 12–18 year olds. Pers Individ Difésa, 36, 75–82.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00053-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cottrell, C. A., Neuberg, S. L., & Li, N. P. (2007). What do people desire in others? A sociofunctional perspective on the importance of different valued characteristics. J Pers Soc Psychol, 92, 208–231.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.208.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Crocetti, E., Klimstra, T. A., Hale, III, W. W., Koot, H. M., & Meeus, W. (2013). Impact of early adolescent externalizing problem behaviors on identity development in middle to late adolescence: a prospective 7-year longitudinal study. J Youth Adolesc, 42, 1745–1758.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9924-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Crocetti, E., Moscatelli, S., Van der Graaff, J., Rubini, M., Meeus, W., & Branje, S. (2016). The interplay of self-certainty and prosocial development in the transition from late adolescence to emerging adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 30, 594–607.  https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Crocetti, E., Moscatelli, S., Kaniušonytė, G., Branje, S., Žukauskienė, R., & Rubini, M. (2018). Adolescents’ self-perception of morality, competence, and sociability and their Interplay with quality of family, friend, and school relationships: a three-wave longitudinal study. J Youth Adolesc, 47, 1743–1754.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-018-0864-z.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Crocetti, E., Rubini, M., Branje, S., Koot, H. M., & Meeus, W. (2016). Self-concept clarity in adolescents and parents: a six-wave longitudinal and multi-informant study on development and intergenerational transmission. J Pers, 84, 580–593.  https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12181.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: the stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(07)00002-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., & Strycker, L. A. (2006). An introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  25. Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: a social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  26. Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: a multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Dev Psychol, 37, 451–463.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.4.451.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: are self-serving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861–875.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.861.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends Cogn Sci, 11, 77–83.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Fiske, S. T (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice and discrimination. In D. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, G. Lindzey (Eds.) Handbook of social psychology. 4th edn, Vol. 1. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  30. Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., & Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci, 2, 861–863.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. De Goede, I. H. A., Branje, S. J. T., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2009). Developmental changes in adolescents’ perceptions of relationships with their parents. J Youth Adolesc, 38, 75–88.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9286-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Moral character in person perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 38–44.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. J Pers Soc Psychol, 106, 148–168.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034726.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. J Pers Soc Psychol, 101, 366–385.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Harter, S. (1988). The self-perception profile for adolescents. Denver, CO: University of Denver.Google Scholar
  36. Jewell, J. A., & Brown, C. S. (2013). Sexting, catcalls, and butt slaps: how gender stereotypes and perceived group norms predict sexualized behavior. Sex Roles, 69, 594–604.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0320-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kanka, M.H., Wagner, P., Buchmann, M., & Spiel, C. (2017). Gender-stereotyped preferences in childhood and early adolescence: A comparison of cross-sectional and longitudinal data. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1–17.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1365703.
  38. Klimstra, T. A., Hale III., W. W., Raaijmakers, Q. A. W., Branje, S. J. T., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2009). Maturation of personality in adolescence. J Pers Soc Psychol, 96, 898–912.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014746.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Klimstra, T. A., Hale III., W. W., Raaijmakers, Q. A. W., Branje, S. J. T., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2010). Identity formation in adolescence: change or stability? J Youth Adolesc, 39, 150–162.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9401-4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Kroger, J. (1997). Gender and identity: the intersection of structure, content, and context. Sex Roles, 36, 747–770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Landy, J. F., Piazza, J., & Goodwin, G. P. (2016). When it’s bad to be friendly and smart: the desirability of sociability and competence depends on morality. Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 42, 1272–1290.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216655984.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Lapsley, D., & Carlo, G. (2014). Moral development at the crossroads: new trends and possible futures. Dev Psychol, 50, 1–7.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a003522.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Leach, C. W., Bilali, R., & Pagliaro, S. (2015). Groups and morality. In J. Simpson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), APA Handbook of personality and social psychology. Interpersonal Relationships and Group Processes, Vol. 2, (123–149). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  44. Leach, C. W., Carraro, L., Garcia, R. L., & Kang, J. J. (2017). Morality stereotyping as a basis of women’s in-group favoritism: An implicit approach. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 20, 153–172.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430215603462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: the importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. J Pers Soc Psychol, 93, 234–249.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Lerner, R. M. & Steinberg, L. (Eds.) (2009). Handbook of adolescent psychology. 3rd edn. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
  47. Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2017). Understanding and undermining the development of gender dichotomies: the legacy of Sandra Lipsitz Bem. Sex Roles, 76, 544–555.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0519-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc, 83, 1198–1202.  https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  50. Lodi-Smith, J., & Crocetti, E. (2018). Self-concept clarity development across the lifespan. In J. Lodi-Smith & K. G. DeMarree(Eds.), Self-concept clarity: Perspectives on assessment, research, and applications (pp. 67–84). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  51. Maes, M., Vanhalst, J., Spithoven, A. W. M., Van den Noortgate, W., & Goossens, L. (2016). Loneliness and attitudes toward aloneness in adolescence: a person-centered approach. J Youth Adolesc, 45, 547–567.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0354-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Meeus, W. (2011). The study of adolescent identity formation 2000-2010: a review of longitudinal research. J Res Adolesc, 21, 75–94.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00716.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Meeus, W. (2016). Adolescent psychosocial development: a review of longitudinal models and research. Dev Psychol, 52, 1969–1993.  https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000243.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Meeus, W. (2019). Adolescent development. Longitudinal research into self, personal relationships, and psychopathology. London & New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  56. Meeus, W., van de Schoot, R., Keijsers, L., Schwartz, S. J., & Branje, S. (2010). On the progression and stability of adolescent identity formation: a five-wave longitudinal study in early-to-middle and middle-to-late adolescence. Child Dev, 81, 1565–1581.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01492.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Menegatti, M., Crocetti, E., & Rubini, M. (2017). Do gender and ethnicity make the difference? Linguistic evaluation bias in primary school. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 36, 415–437.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X17694980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Menon, M. (2011). Does felt gender compatibility mediate influences of self-perceived gender nonconformity on early adolescents’ psychosocial adjustment? Child Dev, 82, 1152–1162.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01601.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Moscatelli, S., Menegatti, M., Albarello, F., Pratto, F., & Rubini, M. (2019). Can we identify with a nation low in morality? The heavy weight of (im) morality in international comparison. Political Psychology, 40, 93–110.  https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Moscatelli, S., Menegatti, M., Ellemers, N., Mariani, M.G., & Rubini, M. (2018). The Lehman Sisters would not be hired: The ‘Perfection bias’, or rather, the heavy weight of morality and competence hindering women’s selection. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
  61. Moscatelli, S., & Rubini, M. (2011). The impact of group entitativity on linguistic discrimination: Ingroup favoritism and outgroup derogation in the explanation of negative outcome allocations. Soc Psychol, 42, 292–299.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Mroczek, D.K. (2007). The analysis of longitudinal data in personality research. In R.W. Robins, R.C. Fraley & R.F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 543–556). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  63. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus User’s Guide. 7th edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. 2016.Google Scholar
  64. Pagliaro, S., Ellemers, N., Barreto, M., & Di Cesare, C. (2016). Once dishonest, always dishonest? the impact of perceived pervasiveness of moral evaluations of the self on motivation to restore a moral reputation. Front Psychol.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00586. 7(APR).CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Pop, E. I., Negru-Subtirica, O., Crocetti, E., Opre, A., & Meeus, W. (2016). On the interplay between academic achievement and educational identity: a longitudinal study. J Adolesc, 47, 135–144.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.11.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Prati, F., Menegatti, M., Moscatelli, S., Kana Kenfack, C.S., Pireddu, S., Crocetti, E., Mariani, M.G., & Rubini, M. (2018). Are mixed gender committees less biased towards female and male employees? A linguistic analysis of competence, morality and sociability in performance appraisal. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
  67. Prati, F., Moscatelli, S., Van Lange, P. A. M., Van Doesum, N. J., & Rubini, M. (2018). The central role of morality in perceived humanness and unselfish behaviors. Soc Psychol, 49, 330–343.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Prati, F., Vasiljevic, M., Crisp, R. J., & Rubini, M. (2015). Some extended psychological benefits of challenging social stereotypes: decreased dehumanization and a reduced reliance on heuristic thinking. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 18, 801–816.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430214567762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Preacher, K. J. (2010). Latent growth curve models. In G. R. Hancock & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), The reviewer’s guide to quantitative methods in the social sciences (pp. 185–198). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  70. Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: the contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychol Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2001). The kids are alright: growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol, 81, 670–683.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.4.670.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull, 126, 3–25.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2002). The role of honour concerns in emotional reactions to offences. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 143–163.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Rogers, A. A., Ha, T., Updegraff, K. A., & Iida, M. (2018). Adolescents’ daily romantic experiences and negative mood: a dyadic, intensive longitudinal study. J Youth Adolesc, 47, 1517–1530.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0797-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Rubini, M., Moscatelli, S., Albarello, F., & Palmonari, A. (2007). Group power as a determinant of interdependence and intergroup discrimination. Eur J Soc Psychol, 37, 1203–1221.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger, A. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. Annual Review of Psy, 57, 255–284.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Smetana, J. G (2013). Moral development: The Social domain theory view. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.) The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 1): body and mind. (832–863). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  79. Smith, T. E., & Leaper, C. (2006). Self-perceived gender typicality and the peer context during adolescence. J Res Adolesc, 16, 91–103.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2006.00123.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Stewart, A. L., Pratto, F., Bou Zeineddine, F., Sweetman, J., Eicher, V., Licata, L., … van Stekelenburg, J. (2016). International support for the arab uprisings: Understanding sympathetic collective action using theories of social dominance and social identity. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 19, 6–26.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430214558310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Van der Graaff, J., Branje, S., De Wied, M., Hawk, S., Van Lier, P., & Meeus, W. (2014). Perspective taking and empathic concern in adolescence: gender differences in developmental changes. Dev Psychol, 50, 881–888.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034325.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., & Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial behavior in adolescence: gender differences in development and links with empathy. J Youth Adolesc, 47, 1086–1099.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0786-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Van de Schoot, R., Lugtig, P., & Hox, J. (2012). A checklist for testing measurement invariance. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 486–492.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.686740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Van Dongen-Melman, J. E. W. M., Koot, H. M., & Verhulst, F. C. (1993). Cross-cultural validation of Harter's Self-Perception Profile for Children in a Dutch sample. Educ Psychol Meas, 53, 739–753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Van Lange, P. A. M., & Sedikides, C. (1998). Being more honest but not necessarily more intelligent than others: generality and explanations for the Muhammad Ali effect. Eur J Soc Psychol, 28, 675–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull, 140, 1174–1204.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036620.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Weisgram, E. S. (2016). The cognitive construction of gender stereotypes: evidence for the dual pathways model of gender differentiation. Sex Roles, 75, 301–313.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0624-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Ybarra, O., Chan, E., & Park, D. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about morality and competence. Motiv Emot, 25, 85–100.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010633908298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Žukauskienė, R., Raižienė, S., Malinauskienė, O., Gabrialavičiūtė, I., Garckija, R., Vosylis, R., Kaniušonytė, G., Truskauskaitė-Kunevičienė, I., & Kajokienė, I. (2015). Pozityvi jaunimo raida Lietuvoje [Positive Youth Development in Lithuania]. Vilnius: MRU.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elisabetta Crocetti
    • 1
    Email author
  • Silvia Moscatelli
    • 1
  • Goda Kaniušonytė
    • 2
  • Wim Meeus
    • 3
  • Rita Žukauskienė
    • 2
  • Monica Rubini
    • 1
  1. 1.Alma Mater Studiorum Università di BolognaBolognaItaly
  2. 2.Mykolas Romeris UniversityVilniusLithuania
  3. 3.Utrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations