Adolescent Risk Behavior: Differentiating Reasoned And Reactive Risk-taking

Abstract

Although explanatory models of adolescent risk behavior have predominantly focused on adolescents’ limited ability to self-regulate impulsive and/or reward-driven behavior (reactive risk behavior), recent arguments suggest that a significant proportion of adolescent risk behavior may actually be strategic and planned in advance (reasoned risk behavior). The present study evaluates hypothesized predictors of reasoned versus reactive risk behavior using self-reported and neurocognitive task data from a large, diverse adolescent sample (N = 1266 participants; N = 3894 risk behaviors). Participants’ mean age was 16.5 years (SD = 1.1); 56.9% were female, 61.9% White, 17.1% Black, 7.0% Hispanic, and 14.1% other race/ethnicity; 40% were in 10th grade, 60% in 12th grade. As hypothesized, reasoned risk behavior (compared to reactive risk behavior) was associated with higher levels of sensation seeking, better working memory, greater future orientation, and perceiving risk behavior to be more beneficial than risky. These results support the distinction between reasoned and reactive risk behavior as meaningful subtypes of adolescent risk behavior and challenge prevailing frameworks that attribute adolescent risk behavior primarily to poor response inhibition.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

References

  1. Benthin, A., Slovic, P., & Severson, H. (1993). A psychometric study of adolescent risk perception. Journal of Adolescence, 16(2), 153–168. https://doi.org/10.1006/jado.1993.1014.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Berns, G. S., Moore, S., & Capra, C. M. (2009). Adolescent engagement in dangerous behaviors is associated with increased white matter maturity of frontal cortex. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6773 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006773.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(2), 116–122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417738144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187–207. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Boyer, T. W., & Byrnes, J. P. (2009). Adolescent risk-taking: integrating personal, cognitive, and social aspects of judgment. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 23–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.10.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28(1), 62–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Casey, B. J., & Jones, R. M. (2010). Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(12), 1189–1201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS). https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/data/yrbs/index.htm

  9. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016). CDC Wonder [Online databases]. http://wonder.cdc.gov

  10. Chen, P., & Vazsonyi, A. T. (2011). Future orientation, impulsivity, and problem behaviors: a longitudinal moderation model. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1633–1645. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025327.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Cosenza, M., & Nigro, G. (2015). Wagering the future: cognitive distortions, impulsivity, delay discounting, and time perspective in adolescent gambling. Journal of Adolescence, 45, 56–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.08.015.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Crawford, A. M., Pentz, M. A., Chou, C.-P., Li, C., & Dwyer, J. H. (2003). Parallel developmental trajectories of sensation seeking and regular substance use in adolescents. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(3), 179–192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.17.3.179.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Culyba, A. J., Abebe, K. Z., Albert, S. M., Jones, K. A., Paglisotti, T., Zimmerman, M. A., & Miller, E. (2018). Association of future orientation with violence perpetration among male youths in low-resource neighborhoods. JAMA Pediatrics, 172(9), 877–879. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1158.

    PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Donati, M. A., Sottili, E., Morsanyi, K., & Primi, C. (2018). Time perspectives and gambling in adolescent boys: differential effects of present- and future-orientation. Journal of Gambling Studies. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-9780-0. in press.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Durston, S., Thomas, K. M., Worden, M. S., Yang, Y., & Casey, B. J. (2002). The effect of preceding context on inhibition: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 16(2), 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1074.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Fischhoff, B., Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., Millstein, S. G., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2010). Adolescents’ perceived risk of dying. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(3), 265–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.026.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Gibbons, F. X., Houlihan, A. E., & Gerrard, M. (2009). Reason and reaction: the utility of a dual-focus, dual-processing perspective on promotion and prevention of adolescent health risk behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 14(2), 231–248. https://doi.org/10.1348/135910708X376640.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Goldberg, J. H., Halpern-Felsher, B. L., & Millstein, S. G. (2002). Beyond invulnerability: the importance of benefits in adolescents’ decision to drink alcohol. Health Psychology, 21(5), 477–484. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.21.5.477.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Goldberg, J. H., Millstein, S., Schwartz, A., & Halpern-Felsher, B. (2009). Intertemporal tradeoffs: perceiving the risk in the benefits of marijuana in a prospective study of adolescents and young adults. Medical Decision Making, 29(2), 182–192. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X08323918.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Harden, K. P., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2011). Individual differences in the development of sensation seeking and impulsivity during adolescence: further evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 47(3), 739–746. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023279.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child Development, 53(1), 87–97. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Henges, A. L., & Marczinski, C. A. (2012). Impulsivity and alcohol consumption in young social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 37(2), 217–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.09.013.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Heron, M. (2017). Deaths: leading causes for 2015. National Vital Statistics Reports, 66(5), 1–76. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nvsr/nvsr66/nvsr66_05.pdf

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Heitzeg, M. M., Nigg, J. T., Yau, W.-Y. W., Zucker, R. A., & Zubieta, J.-K. (2010). Striatal dysfunction marks preexisting risk and medial prefrontal dysfunction is related to problem drinking in children of alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 68(3), 287–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.020.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 298–306. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 962–977. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012705.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, R. L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 401–414. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Jackman, D. M., & MacPhee, D. (2017). Self-esteem and future orientation predict adolescents’ risk engagement. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 37(3), 339–366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431615602756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Johnson, S. R. L., Blum, R. W., & Cheng, T. L. (2014). Future orientation: a construct with implications for adolescent health and wellbeing. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 26(4), 459–468. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijamh-2013-0333.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M., & Hurt, H. (2012). Early adolescent sexual debut: the mediating role of working memory ability, sensation seeking, and impulsivity. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1416–1428. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027491.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M., & Hurt, H. (2013). Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: the mediational role of impulsivity. Addiction, 108(3), 506–515. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12001.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M., & Hurt, H. (2015). Stronger working memory reduces sexual risk taking in adolescents, even after controlling for parental influences. Child Development, 86(4), 1125–1141. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12383.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Kwon, M. S., Vorobyev, V., Moe, D., Parkkola, R., & Hämäläinen, H. (2014). Brain structural correlates of risk-taking behavior and effects of peer influence in adolescents. PLoS One, 9(11), e112780 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112780.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Lennox, R. D., & Cecchini, M. A. (2008). The NARCONON drug education curriculum for high school students: a non-randomized, controlled prevention trial. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 3, 8 https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-597X-3-8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Luciana, M. (2013). Adolescent brain development in normality and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4, Pt 2), 1325–1345. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000643.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Maruska, K., Hansen, J., Hanewinkel, R., & Isensee, B. (2016). The role of substance-specific skills and cognitions in the effectiveness of a school-based prevention program on smoking incidence. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 39(3), 336–355. https://doi.org/10.1177/0163278715588825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Mashhoon, Y., Betts, J., Farmer, S. L., & Lukas, S. E. (2018). Early onset tobacco cigarette smokers exhibit deficits in response inhibition and sustained attention. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 184, 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.11.020.

    PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Maslowsky, J., Buvinger, E., Keating, D. P., Steinberg, L., & Cauffman, E. (2011a). Cost-benefit analysis mediation of the relationship between sensation seeking and risk behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(7), 802–806. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.028.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Maslowsky, J., Keating, D. P., Monk, C. S., & Schulenberg, J. (2011b). Planned versus unplanned risks: neurocognitive predictors of subtypes of adolescents’ risk behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(2), 152–160. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025410378069.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Miech, R. A., Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E., & Patrick, M. E. (2018). Monitoring the future: National survey results on drug use, 1975-2017. Volume 1: Secondary school students. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Institute for Social Research.

  42. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Murphy, S. L., Xu, J., Kochanek, K. D., Curtin, S. C., & Arias, E. (2017). Deaths: final data for 2015. National Vital Statistics Reports, 66(6), 1–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th edn). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.

  45. Nurmi, J.-E. (1991). How do adolescents see their future? A review of the development of future orientation and planning. Developmental Review, 11(1), 1–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(91)90002-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Peeters, M., Janssen, T., Monshouwer, K., Boendermaker, W., Pronk, T., Wiers, R., & Vollebergh, W. (2015). Weaknesses in executive functioning predict the initiating of adolescents’ alcohol use. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 139–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.04.003.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Pomery, E. A., Gibbons, F. X., Reis-Bergan, M., & Gerrard, M. (2009). From willingness to intention: experience moderates the shift from reactive to reasoned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(7), 894–908. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209335166.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Quinn, P. D., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Differential changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking and the escalation of substance use from adolescence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 25(1), 223–239. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000284.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00026.x.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Reyna, V. F., Wilhelms, E. A., McCormick, M. J., & Weldon, R. B. (2015). Development of risky decision making: fuzzy-trace theory and neurobiological perspectives. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 122–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12117.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Robbins, R. N., & Bryan, A. (2004). Relationships between future orientation, impulsive sensation seeking, and risk behavior among adjudicated adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19(4), 428–445. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558403258860.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M., Yang, W., & Hurt, H. (2011). Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1119–1133. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01061.x.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Romer, D., Reyna, V. F., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.007.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Ross, V., Jongen, E., Brijs, T., Ruiter, R., Brijs, K., & Wets, G. (2015). The relation between cognitive control and risky driving in young novice drivers. Applied Neuropsychology, 22(1), 61–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2013.838958.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Sloboda, Z., Pyakuryal, A., Stephens, P. C., Teasdale, B., Forrest, D., Stephens, R. C., & Grey, S. F. (2008). Reports of substance abuse prevention programming available in schools. Prevention Science, 9(4), 276–287. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-008-0102-0.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Steiger, R. M., Stoddard, S. A., & Pierce, J. (2017). Adolescents’ future orientation and nonmedical use of prescription drugs. Addictive Behaviors, 65, 269–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.08.017.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1764–1778. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012955.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80(1), 28–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Stoddard, S. A., Zimmerman, M. A., & Bauermeister, J. A. (2011). Thinking about the future as a way to succeed in the present: a longitudinal study of future orientation and violent behaviors among African American youth. American Journal of Community Psychology, 48(3–4), 238–246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-010-9383-0.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C. S., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Crone, E. A. (2010). What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20(1), 61–69. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp078.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1015.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Woods, D. L., Kishiyamaa, M. M., Lund, E. W., Herron, T. J., Edwards, B., Poliva, O., Hink, R. F., & Reed, B. (2011). Improving digit span assessment of short-term verbal memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(1), 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2010.493149.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: a valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Zuckerman, M. (2007). Sensation seeking and risky behavior. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 10.1037/11555-000.

  66. Zuckerman, M., Eysenck, S. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Sensation seeking in England and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(1), 139–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.46.1.139.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ Contribution

J.M. conceived of the study, conducted the statistical analysis, and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. O.O. assisted in study conception and writing. D.K. and E.D.H. designed and executed the survey. E.D.H. organized data preparation, and assisted in study conception and writing. D.K. assisted in study conception and writing. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Grants R01HD075806 (PI: Keating), K01HD091416 (PI: Maslowsky), and R24HD042849 (to the Population Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin, of which Maslowsky is a faculty affiliate).

Data Sharing and Declaration

The investigators are committed to sharing the data generated through this research, however, data collection is currently ongoing and is not currently publically available. Under the terms of our grant, we intend to make data available to the wider research community within 12 months following the completion of data collection. This includes all self-report, neurocognitive, and imaging parameters which will be included in the database, along with demographic information that does not risk confidentiality

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julie Maslowsky.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study has been granted ethical approval by the University of Michigan Institutional review Board.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from parents of all minor participants included in the study. Minor participants also provided assent to participate. Participants age 18 and older provided informed consent to participate.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maslowsky, J., Owotomo, O., Huntley, E.D. et al. Adolescent Risk Behavior: Differentiating Reasoned And Reactive Risk-taking. J Youth Adolescence 48, 243–255 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-018-0978-3

Download citation

Keywords

  • Risk behavior
  • Sensation seeking
  • Future orientation
  • Working memory
  • Risk/benefit appraisal