The eleven contributions assembled in this special issue have approached the overarching theme of social inequality, life course transitions, and adolescent development from different angles and analytical foci. Common to all contributions is that they are based on longitudinal data collected in different countries. Five contributions are based on data from Germany, one each from Italy and the UK, and four from the USA. Despite the wide variety of individual research questions examined within this conceptual frame, two distinct thematic clusters may be identified. The first cluster of contributions is devoted to transitions in the juvenile life course, examining either the social and individual prerequisites of successful transitions or the consequences of the transition for adolescents’ further development. Contributions belonging to the second cluster are primarily interested in the mechanisms by which (dis-)advantages associated with social background are transmitted from parents to their adolescent offspring. Two further contributions complete the special issue by offering alternative perspectives on the socialization of agency in non-familial relationship contexts of growing-up.
Transitions as Major Challenges in the Adolescent Life Course—Prerequisites and Consequences
The first cluster of articles has in common that they conceptualize life course transitions as “calls to action.” This implies that transitions are personally challenging life events, structuring and directing individual pathways through the adolescent life stage. The contribution by Holtmann, Menze and Solga and that by Ng-Knight and Schoon focus on the agentic and social prerequisites affecting young people’s coping with life course transitions. They show how young people select themselves into particular pathways and associated social environments. Benner, Boyle, and Bakhtiari and Titzmann and Jugert direct their attention to the consequences of major transition experiences for adolescent development, emphasizing that transitions are challenging because they come along with fundamental changes in the social environments framing and regulating the growth of agency. The authors’ main interest is in the adaption processes following transition experiences, and the social and individual factors helping young people to manage the changes in their lives in a “successful” way likely to boost further agentic development.
Demanding transitions: Agentic prerequisites of major status passages
Holtmann
et al. and NgKnight and Schoon share the aim to identify agentic resources that enable disadvantaged or even “low achieving” youth to manage the transition into vocational education and training or the labor market. In Holtmann
et al.’s study, based on data from the large German National Education Panel Study (NEPS), “low achievers” are those holding the lowest or no lower-secondary educational degrees. This contribution is unique in making a point of the tensions between individual agency expressed in search and application activities and the social constraints limiting individual efficacy. These constraints are conceptualized as stigmatization by employers based on young people’s low school achievement. The within-group perspective permits the authors to consider often neglected heterogeneity among these individuals in the realms of cognitive and non-cognitive skills as well as network resources and career plans. The contribution by NgKnight and Schoon, using longitudinal data from the LYPSE study (UK), focus on young people’s transition out of secondary schooling into a status commonly described as “not in education, employment or training” (NEET). The authors ask whether social background and internal locus of control jointly affect the duration of being NEET. In particular, they provide assumptions about independent effects, cumulative advantages as well as compensatory effects of the two factors in protecting young people against staying NEET for a precariously prolonged time. This contribution alerts us to the complexity of interactions and path dependencies among all factors of the “trident” of social inequality, life course transitions, and adolescent development, as it considers the possibility that agentic resources matter differently for youth from diverse social backgrounds.
Consequential transitions: adolescent development in view of new social contexts
In the US-American context, the transition to high school is a challenging status passage in the adolescent life stage. A key question is how the associated transformation of young people’s proximal social networks (e.g., peers) relates to adolescent adjustment, for example, to new performance requirements. Benner
et al. start from the idea that maintaining supportive relationships across this challenging transition phase may buffer against imminent loss of well-being, school engagement, and achievement. Making use of short-term longitudinal data, the authors provide a dense illustration of linked dynamics in young people’s social relationships and socioemotional (mal-)adjustment while entering novel settings of opportunities and constraints related to the role of being a high-school-student. Titzmann and Jugert go beyond the conceptualization of transitions as institutionalized status passages. They examine how migration to a new country, a fundamental biographical transition likely to transform young people’s frameworks for self-evaluation, affects the development of self-efficacy, one of the most prominent indicators of agency. Focusing on the group of ethnic German diaspora immigrants, the authors advance hypotheses about temporal as well as persistent implications of transition experiences for adolescent personal growth. Using longitudinal data including four measurement time points, this contribution is designed to illuminate both short-term effects of migration on adolescents’ level of self-efficacy and extended effects on developmental trajectories following the transition event.
Intergenerational Transmission of Social Inequality—A Multitude of Transmission Belts
As noted by Schulz and coauthors in their contribution to this special issue, most sociological research has focused on the role of socio-economic differences in the intergenerational reproduction of educational and status inequality, thus giving conceptual preference to inequality in various types of social resources. It is particularly noteworthy that all five contributions assembled in the second cluster have gone beyond this well-established transmission mechanism. Four contributions focus instead on whether various components of adolescent agency act as the transition belt in the social reproduction of inequality. They provide assumptions of how social background is related to child and adolescent cognitive and non-cognitive skills and how these, in turn, affect the adolescent status outcome of interest, whereby educational and status attainment in late adolescence or early adulthood is of primary interest. This approach to investigating the social reproduction of inequality takes the social positioning of the family of origin as the starting point and the adolescent or young adult status attainment as the outcome. We may therefore qualify this approach as focusing on the structural level of the intergenerational transmission of social advantages. The contributions by Alessandri, Zuffianò, Eisenberg, and Pastorelli; Kay, Shane, and Heckhausen; Ryberg, Bauldry, Schultz, Steinhoff, and Shanahan; and Schulz, Schunk, Diewald, and Johnson follow this conceptual pattern, focusing on adolescents’ cognitive and non-cognitive skills as transition belts in this process. The fifth contribution, the one by Johnson and Hitlin, is unique in that it raises explicitly the question of the intergenerational transmission of agentic capacities from parents to their adolescent offspring. In this contribution, the intergenerational transmission is situated at the level of agency.
Intergenerational Transmission of Status: Adolescent Agency as Transmission Belt
Alessandri
et al. examine the significance of ego-resiliency,a self-regulatory component characterized by high flexibility and perseverance in the face of adversity, as a potential transmission belt between social background and educational achievement. The Italian longitudinal dataset used for the analysis captures the crucial educational period of the transition from lower-secondary education (i.e., junior high school) to upper-secondary schooling (i.e., senior high school). Adolescents of higher social background may have better-developed ego-resiliency and are thus better equipped to cope with the challenging transition period from lower-to upper-secondary schooling, which, in turn, shows up in better academic performance. While ego-resiliency might be particularly helpful in coping with the challenges of educational tracking, adolescents’ control beliefs, investigated by Kay et al. affect individuals’ goal selection and engagement levels. Based on the German longitudinal SOEP data these authors examine whether control beliefs are the pathways through which the socio-economic status of the family of origin operates for educational attainment in adolescence and status attainment in early adulthood. Starting from the idea that social background is associated with SES-specific belief systems about success in society, the authors argue that such control beliefs are transferred to the offspring in the socialization process. The more adolescents believe that success is under their control, the more likely they are to attain desirable career outcomes in young adulthood (status, income). Yet another transmission belt is proposed in the contribution by Ryberg
et al. These authors investigate the role of adolescent personality traits (i.e., Big Five) for educational attainment in a tracked educational system (i.e., Germany). In line with the overall aim of this special issue, the authors relate the role of personality for educational attainment to the institutionalized educational trajectories marked by transitions (i.e., tracking). They argue that personality traits may be most influential for educational attainment at key turning points in the educational trajectories, as such transitions quasi act as “call to action.” The contribution by Schulz
et al., based on the German TwinLife study (including siblings), acts in some ways as a bridge between the studies focusing on the structural level of the intergenerational transmission of inequality and the study by Johnson and Hitlin addressing the level of agency. Schulz
et al. investigate cognitive ability as the transmission belt between social background and educational attainment in adolescence (i.e., selection to the academic track). They make use of the twin data to decompose the genetic influence on adolescent cognitive ability from the impact of the socioeconomic components of social background, then examining the respective impact on educational attainment. As they observe a strong genetic influence on adolescent cognitive ability and an impact of the shared environment on academic tracking, we may conclude that intergenerational transmission of inequality not only takes place at the structural level (i.e., intergenerational status transmission), but also at the agentic level—in this case, the genetic transmission of cognitive ability.
Intergenerational Development: The Transmission of Agentic Orientations Across Generations
Whether parental agentic orientations are transferred from parents to their adolescent offspring is the key question examined in the contribution by Johnson and Hitlin. It is seldom the case that longitudinal studies provide prospective data on agentic capacity for both parents and their children (i.e., multigenerational panel data). Johnson and Hitlin grasp this unique opportunity provided by a US data set to investigate what they call intergenerational development: Are agentic orientations held by parents when they were adolescents transmitted to their children when they are in the same life stage (i.e., adolescence)?
This approach alerts us to the intriguing question of whether the intergenerational transmission of agentic capacities plays a more prominent role in the social reproduction of inequality than previously assumed. Given the scarcity of multigenerational panel data, there is hardly any empirical evidence of transmission belts running from parental agentic capacities to parental SES and, finally, to offspring’s agency and his or her status attainment. We may easily imagine that, for example, control beliefs about success in society held by parents when they were adolescents impacts their adult status attainment and is subsequently transmitted to their adolescent offspring, which turn affects offspring’s status attainment in adulthood. Data informing about both parents’ and offspring’s developmental and life course trajectories would allow us to disentangle the share of intergenerational transmission that is related to the direct transfer of social advantages (i.e., wealth) from the social opportunities provided by the economic, cultural, and social resources accrued in the family of origin and from the conveyance of agentic capacities. This last point also alludes to the possibility of moderating effects of adolescent agentic capacities on status attainment—an issue that has not been often addressed in the literature. Although adolescent agency components act as transition belts through which social background operates, the associations are far from being perfect as the contributions in this special issue show. Hence, the question arises whether adolescents from lower social background would profit disproportionally more from having acquired these agentic capacities and, vice versa, whether adolescents from higher social background would be disadvantaged less from the lack of such agentic capacities as this lack may be compensated by other advantages linked to their social background? Ryberg and co-authors discuss this possibility in their contribution to the special issue.
Developing Adolescent Agency: The Role of Intra- and Extra-Familial Social Contexts
All nine contributions described above consider that unequal chances for successful goal striving and productive coping with transition experiences may be related to disparities in young people’s proximal social environments, family background in particular. Two further contributions take a different perspective. Wang, Champine, Ferris, Hershberg, Warren, Burkhard, Su, and Lerner’s interest focuses on how adolescent agency may be developed by non-familial social environments. Mulvey and Killen ask how children and adolescents themselves contribute to the formation of their out-of-home and out-of-school social contexts of growing up.
Wang
et al. study the role of extra-familial, extra-curricular interventions in the socialization of agentic capacities. The authors examine how out-of-school programs (here: Boy Scouts of America) can intervene to support socially marginalized youths’ development of what the authors call “character”. This is an umbrella concept involving, for example, cheerfulness, hopeful future expectations, honesty and responsibility. Several of these factors may be pertinent to adolescent agency as addressed in the other contributions to this special issue. While hopeful future expectations are likely to encourage agentic engagement and goal striving (see e.g., Johnson and Hitlin), other aspects may facilitate supportive social bonds and thus bolster the adolescent repertoire of social resources, helping to cope with incisive transition experiences (see e.g., Benner
et al.). Mulvey and Killen zoom in on such social bonds in the realm of the peer environment. They examine how children and adolescents think about the feasibility of challenging and changing different kinds of group norms (i.e., relational and physical aggression as well as unequal resource allocation). By asking how moral thinking about others’ welfare and the liability of group loyalty inform young people’s relational activities with peers, this study provides insights into the contextual and individual factors influencing the formation of young people’s out-of-home and out-of-school environments pertinent to the socialization of agency.