Skip to main content
Log in

The Different Benefits from Different Gestures in Understanding a Concept

  • Published:
Journal of Science Education and Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Explanations are typically accompanied by hand gestures. While research has shown that gestures can help learners understand a particular concept, different learning effects in different types of gesture have been less understood. To address the issues above, the current study focused on whether different types of gestures lead to different levels of improvement in understanding. Two types of gestures were investigated, and thus, three instructional videos (two gesture videos plus a no gesture control) of the subject of mitosis—all identical except for the types of gesture used—were created. After watching one of the three videos, participants were tested on their level of understanding of mitosis. The results showed that (1) differences in comprehension were obtained across the three groups, and (2) representational (semantic) gestures led to a deeper level of comprehension than both beat gestures and the no gesture control. Finally, a language proficiency effect is discussed as a moderator that may affect understanding of a concept. Our findings suggest that a teacher is encouraged to use representational gestures even to adult learners, but more work is needed to prove the benefit of using gestures for adult learners in many subject areas.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the current study, participants who did not complete their secondary schooling taught in English were regarded as non-native English speakers.

  2. None of the participants had trouble with the instructions given during the experiment.

  3. For the drawing test scores, an interrater reliability for the raters was found to be κ = 0.74 (p < 0.01), which is a substantial agreement.

References

  • Ainsworth S, Loizou AT (2003) The effects of self-explaining when learning with text or diagrams. Cogn Sci 27:669–681

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali MW, Heath DC, Myers HJ (2001) Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: some gestures are meant to be seen. J Mem Lang 44:169–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas JB (1994) Gestures as part of speech: methodological implications. Res Lang Soc Interact 27:201–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beattie G, Shovelton H (1999) Do iconic hand gestures really contribute anything to the semantic information conveyed by speech? An experimental investigation. Semiotica 123:1–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger D (1983) Intonation and gesture. Am Speech 58:156–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butcher KR (2006) Learning from text with diagrams: promoting mental model development and inference generation. J Educ Psychol 98:182–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassell J, Bickmore T, Billinghurst M, Campbell L, Chang K, Vilhjalmsson H, Yan H (1999) Embodiment in conversational interfaces: rea. In: Proceedings of computer-human interaction, Pittsburgh, pp 520–527

  • Cavé C, Guaïtella I, Bertrand R, Santi S, Harlay F, Espesser R (1996) About the relationship between eyebrow movement and F0 variations. In: Proceedings of the international conference on spoken language processing (ICSLP), Philadelphia, pp 2175–2179

  • Chase WG, Simon HA (1973) The mind’s eye in chess. In: Chase WG (ed) Visual information processing. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi MTH, Roscoe R, Slotta J, Roy M, Chase M (2012) Misconceived causal explanations for “emergent” processes. Cogn Sci 36:1–61

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church RB, Goldin-Meadow S (1986) The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition 23:43–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Church RB, Kelly SD, Lynch K (2000) Immediate memory for mismatched speech and representational gesture across development. J Nonverbal Behav 24:151–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church RB, Ayman-Nolley S, Estrada J, Glover D, Dullum T (2001) What role can gesture play in teaching mathematical concepts? Paper presented at the society for research in child development, Minneapolis

  • Church RB, Ayman-Nolley S, Mahootian S (2004) The role of gesture in bilingual education: does gesture enhance learning? Int J Biling Educ Biling 7:303–319

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen AA (1977) The communicative functions of hand illustrators. J Commun 27:54–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook SW, Goldin-Meadow S (2006) The role of gesture in learning: do children use their hands to change their minds? J Cogn Dev 7:211–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler A (1984) Stress and accent in language production and understanding. In: Gibbon D, Richter H (eds) Intonation, accent and rhythm studies in discourse phonology. De Gruyter Berllin, pp 77–90

  • de Groot AD (1966) Perception and memory versus thought: Some old ideas and recent findings. In: Kleinmuntz B (ed) Problem solving. Wiley, New York, p 1966

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S, Kim S, Singer M (1999) What the teacher’s hands tell the student’s mind about math. J Educ Psychol 91:720–730

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham JA, Argyle M (1975) A cross-cultural study of the communication of extra-verbal meaning by gestures. Int J Psychol 10:57–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter AB, Alibali MW, Kita S (2007) I see it in my hands’ eye: representational gestures reflect conceptual demands. Lang Cogn Process 22:313–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibáñez A, Manes F, Escobar J, Trujillo N, Andreucci P, Hurtado E (2010) Gesture influences the processing of figurative language in non-native speakers: ERP evidence. Neurosci Lett 471:48–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly SD, Church B (1998) A comparison between children’s and adult’s ability to detect conceptual information conveyed through representational gestures. Child Dev 69:85–93

    Google Scholar 

  • Krahmer E, Swerts M (2007) The effects of visual beats on prosodic prominence: acoustic analyses, auditory perception and visual perception. J Mem Lang 57:396–414

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss RM, Chen Y, Chawla P (1996) Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal communication: What do conversational hand gestures tell us? In: Zanna M (ed) Advances in experimental social psychology. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 389–450

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer RE, Heiser J, Lonn S (2001) Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: when presenting more materials results in less understanding. J Educ Psychol 93:187–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeil NM, Alibali MW, Evans JL (2000) The role of gesture in children’s comprehension of spoken language: now they need it, now they don’t. J Nonverbal Behav 24:131–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill D (1992) Hand and mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan B (1953) Question melodies in American English. Am Speech 2:181–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ping R, Goldin-Meadow S (2008) Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Dev Psychol 44:1277–1287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riseborough MG (1981) Physiographic gestures as decoding facilitators: three experiments exploring a neglected facet of communication. J Nonverbal Behav 5:172–183

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers WT (1978) The contribution of kinetic illustrators toward the comprehension of verbal behavior within utterances. Human Commun Res 5:54–62

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer MA, Goldin-Meadow S (2005) Children learn when their teachers’ gestures and speech differ. Psychol Sci 16:85–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J (1988) Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn Sci 12:257–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J (1993) Some cognitive processes and their consequences for the organisation and presentation of information. Aust J Psychol 45:1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terken J, Nooteboom S (1987) Opposite effects of accentuation and deaccentuation on verification latencies for given and new information. Lang Cogn Process 2:145–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tfouni LV, Klatzky RL (1983) A discourse analysis of deixis: pragmatic, cognitive and semantic factors in the comprehension of ‘this’, ‘that’, ‘here’, and ‘there’. J Child Lang 10:123–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson LA, Massaro DW (1994) Children’s integration of speech and pointing gestures in comprehension. J Exp Child Psychol 57:327–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson LA, Driscoll D, Markson L (1998) Memory for visual-spoken language in children and adults. J Nonverbal Behav 22:167–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E, Thompson DM (1973) Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychol Rev 80:352–373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky B (1999) What does drawing reveal about thinking? In: Gero JS, Tversky B (eds) Visual and spatial reasoning in design. Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, Sydney, pp 93–101

    Google Scholar 

  • Valenzeno L, Alibali MW, Klatzky R (2003) Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: a lesson in symmetry. Contemp Educ Psychol 28:187–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins NJ, Rawson KA (2011) Controlling retrieval during practice: implications for memory-based theories of automaticity. J Mem Lang 65:208–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodall WG, Folger JP (1981) Encoding specificity and nonverbal cue context: an expansion of episodic memory research. Commun Monogr 49:39–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodall WG, Folger JP (1985) Nonverbal cue context and episodic memory: on the availability and endurance of nonverbal behaviors as retrieval cues. Commun Monogr 52:319–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Seokmin Kang.

Appendices

Appendix 1

A script used in the representational gesture video

Letters in parentheses are a gesture type that represents following words.

  • d: deictic gesture

  • i: iconic gesture

  • m: metaphoric

  • on: on-diagram gesture

  • off: off-diagram gesture

  • for example, “… (d-on) chromatin…” was presented with a speaker’s sweeping a word “chromatin” with his index finger on the diagram.

I am going to explain about nuclear division, known as (d-on) “mitosis”.

Cell division is a process that enables organisms to grow and reproduce. Dividing cells go through an ordered series of events called the cell cycle.

Mitosis is a phase of the cell cycle in which the genetic material from a parent cell is divided equally between two daughter cells. Before a dividing cell enters mitosis, it undergoes a period of growth called (d-on) interphase. Some 90 % of a cell’s time in the normal cellular cycle may be spent in interphase. The cell still has (d-on) nucleoli present during interphase. It is also important to note that the nucleus is bounded by a (i-on) nuclear envelope and the cell’s chromosomes have duplicated but are in the form of (d-on) chromatin. In (d-on) prophase, the chromatin condenses into discrete chromosomes. The (i-on) nuclear envelope (i-on) breaks down and spindles form at (i-off) opposite “poles” of the cell. During prophase, the (d-on) nucleoli (m-off) disappear and the chromatid structure of the (d-on) chromosomes becomes apparent.

Many consider (d-on) prophase, as opposed to interphase, to be the first true step of the mitotic process. A change that occurs in a cell during prophase is that (d-on) chromatin fibers (i-on) become coiled into chromosomes with each chromosome having two chromatids joined at a (d-on, i-off) centromere. Also, two (d-on) centrosomes appears, formed from the replication of one pair in interphase (d-on). Also, the two pairs of centrioles within the (d-on) centrosome move away from one another toward (i-off) opposite ends of the cell due to the lengthening of the microtubules that form between them. In late prophase, the (d-on) nuclear envelope (i-off) breaks up. Polar fibers, c, travel from each cell pole to the (i-on) cell’s equator. The chromosomes begin to migrate (i-on) toward the cell center. In (d-on) metaphase, the spindle (i-off) fully develops and the chromosomes align at the (i-on) metaphase plate—a plane that is equally distant from the (d-on) two spindle poles. A change that takes occurs in a cell during metaphase is that the (d-on) nuclear envelope (m-off) disappears completely. Polar fibers, which, as we said are microtubules that make up the spindle fibers, continue to (d-on) extend from (i-off) opposite poles to the center of the cell (d-on). Chromosomes move randomly until they attach to polar fibers from both sides of their (d-on) centromeres. Chromosomes are held at the metaphase plate by the equal forces of the polar fibers pushing on the centromeres of the chromosomes. In (d-on) anaphase, the paired chromosomes or sister chromatids (d-on, i-off) separate and begin moving to opposite ends of the cell. Spindle fibers not connected to chromatids (i-off) lengthen and elongate the cell. In preparation for telophase, the two cell poles (i-off) move further apart during the course of anaphase. At the end of anaphase, each pole contains a complete compilation of chromosomes. In (d-on) telophase, the (d-on) chromosomes are (i-off) cordoned off into new distinct nuclei in the emerging daughter cells. The following are changes that occur in a cell during telophase. The polar fibers continue to lengthen. The (d-on) pair of nucleoli reappears. Also, chromatin fibers of (d-on) chromosomes uncoil. After these changes, mitosis cycle is largely complete and the genetic “contents” of one cell have been divided (i-off) equally into two. Finally, we return to the (d-on) interphase stage. A new, separate nucleus is produced.

Appendix 2

Example questions of each test

  • Retention question

Example 1

When the chromatids are separated and move to the opposite poles, what is the chromatids called?

Example 2

Some 90 % of a cell’s time in the normal cellular cycle may be spent in this phase.

A. interphase B. prophase C. metaphase D. telophase

  • Immediate transfer question

Example

When the DNA in a cell is uncoiled and spread throughout the nucleus, it is called

A. chromosomes B. chromatids C. centromeres D. chromatin

  • What-if question

Example

If microtubules were eliminated from prophase, what would be the direct influence on the mitosis process?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kang, S., Hallman, G.L., Son, L.K. et al. The Different Benefits from Different Gestures in Understanding a Concept. J Sci Educ Technol 22, 825–837 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-012-9433-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-012-9433-5

Keywords

Navigation