Skip to main content
Log in

Working Memory, Motivation, and Teacher-Initiated Learning

  • Published:
Journal of Science Education and Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Working memory is where we “think” as we learn. A notion that emerges as a synthesis from several threads in the research literatures of cognition, motivation, and connectionism is that motivation in learning is the process whereby working memory resource allocation is instigated and sustained. This paper reviews much literature on motivation and working memory, and concludes that the apparent novelty of the proposal offered to describe motivation in terms of working memory results from the apparent lack of cross-channel exchange among these research traditions. The relation between working memory and motivation is explored in the context of the interactive compensatory model of learning (ICML) in which learning is considered to result from the interaction of ability, motivation, and prior learning. The ICML is recast in light of the revised definition of motivation offered here. This paper goes on to suggest ways in which a range of teaching and learning issues and activities may be reconceptualized in the context of a model emphasizing a learner's working memory that makes use of chunks of previously acquired knowledge.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andrade, J. (2001). The working memory model: Consensus, controversy, and future directions. In Andrade, J. (Ed.), Working Memory in Perspective, Psychology Press, East Sussex, pp. 281- -310.

  • Ayres, P. (1993). Why goal free problems can facilitate learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology 18: 376–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science 255: 556–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 417– 423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. VIII, Academic Press, New York, pp. 47–90.

  • Baddeley, A. D., and Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory. In Models of Working Memory, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 28–61.

  • Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and self-regulatory mechanisms. In Dienstbier, R. A. (Ed.), Perspectives on Motivation: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 38, University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NE, pp. 69–164.

  • Beilock, S. L., and Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail. Working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science 16: 101–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S. (1976). Human Characterisitics and School Learning, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S. (1984). The two sigma problem: The search for methods of groups instruction as effective as one-to-one tutoring. Educational Researcher 13: 4–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, D. W., Nolan, D. E., and Gallagher, S. J. (2003). Automated testing. Journal of Science Education and Technology 12: 183–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R. L., and Bickhard, M. H. (1986). Knowing Levels and Developmental Stages, Karger, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M. H., and LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science 18: 439–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, R. C., and Mayer, R. E. (2003). E-Learning and the Science of Instruction, Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colom, R., Rebollo, I., Palacios, A., Juan-Espinosa, M., and Kyllonen, P. C. (2004). Working memory is (almost) perfectly predicted by g. Intelligence 32: 277–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G. (1998). Research into cognitive load theory and instructional design at unsw. Retrieved 1/01, from http://www.arts.unsw.edu.au/education/CLT_NET_Aug_97.HTML

  • Cooper, G., and Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology 79: 347–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded process model of working memory. In Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of Working Memory, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 62–101.

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Abuhamdeh, S., and Nakamura, J. (2005). Flow. In Elliott, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of Competence and Motivation, Guilford, New York, pp. 598–608.

  • Dai, D. Y., and Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (2004). Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dick, W., Carey, L., and Carey, J. O. (2001). The Systematic Design of Instruction, 5th ed., Longman, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dochy, P., Segers, M., and Buehl, M. M. (1999). The relation between assessment practices and outcomes of studies: The case of research on prior knowledge. Review of Educational Research 69: 145–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., and Leggett, E. S. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review 95: 256–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, A. J., and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.) (2005). Handbook of Competence and Motivation, Guilford, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. C., and Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In Fiedler, K. and Forgas, J. (Eds.), Affect, Cognition and Social Behavior, C. J. Hogrefe, Toronto, pp. 25–43.

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., and Karmiloff-sm. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

  • Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., and Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of Working Memory, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

  • Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance. In Ericssonn, K. A. (Ed.), The Road to Excellence: The Acquisition of Expert Performance in the Arts, Science and Games, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 1–50.

  • Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., and Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science 208: 1181–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., and Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review 100: 363–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Björk-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A., Nordborg, C., Peterson, D. A., et al. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Medicine 4: 1313–1317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman Barrett, L., Tugade, M. M., and Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin 130: 553–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J. (1999). How Brains Make up Their Minds, Weidenfeld & Nicolson, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, T. (1995). The role of motivational strategies in self-regulated learning. In Pintrich, P. R. (Ed.), Understanding Self-Regulated Learning, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 29–42.

  • Granat, J. P. (2005). Stay in the zone.Com. Retrieved March 25, 2005, from http://www.stayinthezone.com/default_NONFLASHad.htm

  • Hadwin, A. F., and Winne, P. H. (1996). Study strategies have meager support. Journal of Higher Education 67: 692–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1992). Understanding g in terms of information processing. Educational Psychology Review 4: 271–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. (2005). Everything Bad is Good for You: How Today’s Pop Culture is Actually Making us Smarter, Penguin, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H. (1991). Why is science difficult to learn? Things are seldom what they seem. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 7: 75–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., and Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology General 30: 169–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J. M. (1987). The systematic process of motivational design. Performance and Instruction 26: 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., and Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science 15: 661–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (1998). Cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational aspects of problem solving. Cognitive Science 26: 49–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia Learning, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., and Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist 38: 43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human Motivation, Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meinhardt, J., and Pekrun, R. (2003). Additional resource allocation to emotional events: An erp study. Cognition and Emotion 17: 477–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M. M., Nelson, R. J., Stryker, M. P., Cynader, M. S., Schoppmann, A., and Zook, J. M. (2004). Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 224: 591– 605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63: 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (1999). Models of Working Memory, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

  • Moore, J. W., Brooks, D. W., Fuller, R. G., and Jensen, D. D. (1977). Repeatable testing. Journal of Chemical Education 54: 276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, G. R., Ross, S. M., and Kemp, J. E. (2004). Designing Effective Instruction, 4th ed., John Wiley and Sons, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolls, J. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review 91: 328–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (2004). Emotional Design, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, E., and Sweller, J. (1985). What do students learn while solving mathematics problems? Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 272–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 429–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., Renkl, A., and Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist 38: 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas, F., and van Merrienboer, J. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., and Cross, D. R. (1983). Ordinary learning: Pragmatic connections among children’s beliefs, motives, and actions. In Bisanz, J., Bisanz, G., and Kali, R. (Eds.), Learning in Children, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 137–169.

  • Pintrich, P. R., Roeser, R. W., and DeGroot, E. A. M. (1994). Classroom and individual differences in early adolescent’s motivation and self-regulated learning. Journal of Early Adolescence 14: 139–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., and Schunk, D. H. (1996). Motivation in Education: Theory, Research, and Applications, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, P. T. (Ed.). (2003). Connectionist Models of Development: Developmental Processes in Real and Artificial Neural Networks, Psychology Press, New York.

  • Reed, J. H., Hagen, A. S., Wicker, F. W., and Schallert, D. L. (1996). Involvement as a temporal dynamic: Affective factors in studying for exams. Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 101–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, J. H., Schallert, D. L., and Deithloff, L. F. (2002). Investigating the interface between self-regulation and involvement processes. Educational Psychologist 37: 53–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., and Atkinson, R. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skill acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist 38: 15–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Atkinson, R. K., and Grosse, C. S. (2004). How fading worked solution steps works—a cognitive load perspective. Instructional Science 32: 59–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H. (1983). Ability versus effort attributional feedback: Differential effects on self-efficacy and achievement., Journal of Educational Psychology 75: 848–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H. (1991). “Self-efficacy and academic motivation.” Educational Psychologist 26: 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., Brooks, D. W., and Crippen, K. J. (2005). Improving chemistry instruction using an interactive, compensatory model of learning. Journal of Chemical Education 82: 637–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., and Lehman, S. (2001). Situational interest: A review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review 13: 23–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, A. (2004). How including prior knowledge as a subject variable may change outcomes of learning research. American Educational Research Journal 41: 159–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1974). How big is a chunk? Science 183: 482–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. C., Plant, M., Carney, R. N., Arnold, C. S., Jackson, A., Johnson, L. S., et al. (2003). Productivity of educational psychologists in educational psychology journals, 1997–2001. Contemporary Educational Psychology 28: 422–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J. P., and McClelland, J. (2005). Connectionist and dynamic systems approaches to development: On the cusp of a new grand theory or still too distributed?” Retrieved June 10, 2005, from http://www.psychology.uiowa.edu/cd-conference

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2005). Intelligence, competence, and expertise. In Elliott, A. J., and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of Competence and Motivation, Guilford, New York, pp. 15–30.

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review 10: 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taconis, R., Ferguson-Hessler, M. G. M., and Broekkamp, H. (2001). Teaching science problem solving: An overview of experimental work. Journal of Rsch Science Teaching 38: 442–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuovinen, J., and Sweller, J. (1999). A comparison of cognitive load assoicated with discovery learning and worked examples. Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 334–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Merrienboer, J., and Krammer, H. (1987). Instructional strategies and tactics for the design of introductory computer programming courses in high school. Instructional Science 15: 345–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Merrienboer, J., and Krammer, H. (1990). The “completion strategy” in programming instruction: Theoretic and empirical support. In Dijkstra, S., van Hout-Wolters, B. H. M., and van der Sijde, P. C. (Eds.), Research on Instruction, Educational Technology Publications, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, pp. 45–61.

  • Weinstein, C. E., Hanson, G., Powdrill, L., Roska, L., Dierking, D., Husman, J., et al. (1997). The design and evaluation of a course in strategic learning. Retrieved June 13, 2005, from http://www.nade.net/documents/SCP97/SCP97.20.pdf

  • Weinstein, C. E., and Mayer, R. E. (1986). The teaching of learning strategies. In Wittrock, M. C. (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Teaching, 3rd ed., Macmillan, New York, pp. 315–327.

  • Winne, P. H. (1985). Steps toward promoting cognitive achievements. Elementary School Journal 85: 673–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1989). Theories of instruction and of intelligence for designing artificially intelligent tutoring systems. Educational Psychologist 25: 229–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H. (1995). Inherent details in self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist 30: 173–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolters, C. A. (2003). Regulation of motivation: Evaluating an underemphasized aspect of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist 38: 189–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, X., and Simon, H. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing. Cognition and Instruction 4: 137– 166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zielinski, T. J., Brooks, D. W., Crippen, K. J., and March, J. L. (2001). Time and teaching. Journal of Chemical Education 78: 714–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimbardo, P. G. (Ed.). (1969). The Cognitive Control of Motivation, Scott, Foresman and Co., Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., and Schunk, D. H. (2004). Self-regulating intellectual processes and outcomes: A social cognitive perspective. In Dai, D. Y., and Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 323–349.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David W. Brooks.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brooks, D.W., Shell, D.F. Working Memory, Motivation, and Teacher-Initiated Learning. J Sci Educ Technol 15, 17–30 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-006-0353-0

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-006-0353-0

Keywords

Navigation