Advertisement

The Semantic Integration Between Two Subliminally Perceived Words Simultaneously Presented at Different Locations

  • Shen TuEmail author
  • Chengzhen Liu
  • SiShi Zhu
  • Jerwen Jou
  • Yajuan Zhou
  • Simin Wan
Original Research
  • 28 Downloads

Abstract

In the present study, we showed evidence of an integration between two unconscious semantic representations. In experiment 1, two masked Chinese words of the same or different categories (“orange apple” or “grape hammer”) were simultaneously presented in the prime, followed by two Chinese words also of same or different categories in the target. We examined possible prime/target visual feature priming, semantic category priming and motor response priming effects. Moreover, two ISI intervals (53, 163 ms) between the prime and the target words were used to examine the positive and negative priming. The results revealed a negative motor response priming and a positive semantic category priming effect independent of the ISI when the target words were of the same category. Experiment 2 eliminated an alternative interpretation of the effect based on different number of category words changed across the prime and the target. Experiment 3 eliminated a potential confound of unequal numbers of trials for motor congruent and incongruent conditions in Experiment 1. Overall, these results indicated an integration between the meanings of the two subliminally perceived words in the prime. The difference between simultaneous and sequential presentations, and the reason why positive priming was not observed when the interval between the prime and the target was short were discussed in the context of unconscious semantic integration.

Keywords

Masking Semantic category Motor response priming effect Negative priming Integration between unconscious processes 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a funding for the Start-up Project of Scientific Research on Introducing Talents to Guizhou University of Finance and Economics in 2018 [Grant Number 2019YT020] and the National Natural Science Foundation of China [Grant Number 31500871]. Thank Guang Zhao, Yidan Ma and Jiang Qiu for their valuable reviews and feedbacks on the draft.

Supplementary material

10936_2019_9648_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (151 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (PDF 152 kb)

References

  1. Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000). Parts outweigh the whole (word) in unconscious analysis of meaning. Psychological Science, 11(2), 118–124.Google Scholar
  2. Andersen, R. A., Essick, G. K., & Siegel, R. M. (1985). Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science, 230(4724), 456–458.Google Scholar
  3. Atas, A., Faivre, N., Timmermans, B., Cleeremans, A., & Kouider, S. (2014). Nonconscious learning from crowded sequences. Psychological Science, 25(1), 113–119.Google Scholar
  4. Atas, A., Vermeiren, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2013). Repeating a strongly masked stimulus increases priming and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1422–1430.Google Scholar
  5. Bernat, E., Bunce, S., & Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42(1), 11–34.Google Scholar
  6. Boy, F., Husain, M., & Sumner, P. (2010). Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(24), 11134–11139.Google Scholar
  7. Celeghin, A., Savazzi, S., Barabas, M., Bendini, M., & Marzi, C. A. (2015). Blindsight is sensitive to stimulus numerosity and configuration: Evidence from the redundant signal effect. Experimental Brain Research, 233(5), 1617–1623.Google Scholar
  8. Chen, Z., & Saunders, J. A. (2016). Automatic adjustments toward unseen visual targets during grasping movements. Experimental Brain Research, 234(7), 1–13.Google Scholar
  9. Diaz, M. T., & McCarthy, G. (2007). Unconscious word processing engages a distributed network of brain regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1768–1775.Google Scholar
  10. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.Google Scholar
  11. Faivre, N., & Kouider, S. (2011). Multi-feature objects elicit nonconscious priming despite crowding. Journal of Vision, 11(3), 1–10.Google Scholar
  12. Faivre, N., Mudrik, L., Schwartz, N., & Koch, C. (2014). Multisensory integration in complete unawareness: Evidence from audiovisual congruency priming. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2006–2016.Google Scholar
  13. Kiefer, M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(1), 27–39.Google Scholar
  14. Kiefer, M., & Martens, U. (2010). Attentional sensitization of unconscious cognition: Task sets modulate subsequent masked semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 464–489.Google Scholar
  15. Lin, Z., & Murray, S. O. (2014). Unconscious processing of an abstract concept. Psychological Science, 25(1), 296–298.Google Scholar
  16. Liu, C., Sun, Z., Jou, J., Cui, Q., Zhao, G., Qiu, J., et al. (2016). Unconscious processing of facial emotional valence relation: Behavioral evidence of integration between subliminally perceived stimuli. Plos One, 11(9), e162689.Google Scholar
  17. Luck, S. J., Vogel, E. K., & Shapiro, K. L. (1996). Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature, 383, 616–618.Google Scholar
  18. Montoro, P. R., Luna, D., & Ortells, J. J. (2014). Subliminal gestalt grouping: Evidence of perceptual grouping by proximity and similarity in absence of conscious perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 1–8.Google Scholar
  19. Mudrik, L., Faivre, N., & Koch, C. (2014). Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9), 488–496.Google Scholar
  20. Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424.Google Scholar
  21. Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2001). Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli. Cognition, 80(3), 215–229.Google Scholar
  22. Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic Processes in Reading: Visual Word Recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  23. Ortells, J. J., Kiefer, M., Castillo, A., Megías, M., & Morillas, A. (2016). The semantic origin of unconscious priming: Behavioral and event-related potential evidence during category congruency priming from strongly and weakly related masked words. Cognition, 146, 143–157.Google Scholar
  24. Pinto, Y., van Gaal, S., de Lange, F. P., Lamme, V. A., & Seth, A. K. (2015). Expectations accelerate entry of visual stimuli into awareness. Journal of Vision, 15(8), 13.Google Scholar
  25. Rohaut, B., Alario, F. X., Meadow, J., Cohen, L., & Naccache, L. (2016). Unconscious semantic processing of polysemous words is not automatic. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2016(1), niw10.Google Scholar
  26. Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., & Hennighausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38(2), 165–174.Google Scholar
  27. Sklar, A. Y., Levy, N., Goldstein, A., Mandel, R., Maril, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2012). Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(48), 19614–19619.Google Scholar
  28. Stock, A., Friedrich, J., & Beste, C. (2016). Subliminally and consciously induced cognitive conflicts interact at several processing levels. Cortex, 85, 75–89.Google Scholar
  29. Sumner, P. (2007). Negative and positive masked-priming - implications for motor inhibition. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1–2), 317–326.Google Scholar
  30. Szczepanowski, R., Traczyk, J., Wierzchoń, M., & Cleeremans, A. (2013). The perception of visual emotion: Comparing different measures of awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1), 212–220.Google Scholar
  31. Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 54(2), 321–343.Google Scholar
  32. Tu, S. (2014). The relationship between conscious and unconscious processes - The interactions between unconscious processes and the top-down continuous modulations on partial awareness processes. Saarbrücken: Lambert Acedemic Publishing.Google Scholar
  33. Tu, S., Martens, U., Zhao, G., Pan, W., Wang, T., Qiu, J., et al. (2013). Subliminal faces with different valence: Unconscious mismatch detection indicates interactions between unconscious processing. World Journal of Neuroscience, 3(4), 298–306.Google Scholar
  34. Tu, S., & Zhao, G. (2014). A way of integration: The relationship between conscious and unconscious processes. Advances in Psychology, 4(3), 373–384.Google Scholar
  35. Tu, S., Zhu, S., Liang, Q., Jou, J., Wan, S., Zhao, G., et al. (2019). Unconscious integration of sequentially presented subliminal arrow pointing directions. Australian Journal of Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12252.
  36. Valdés, B., Catena, A., & Marí-Beffa, P. (2005). Automatic and controlled semantic processing: A masked prime-task effect. Consciousness & Cognition, 14(2), 278–295.Google Scholar
  37. van Gaal, S., Naccache, L., Meuwese, J. D. I., van Loon, A. M., Leighton, A. H., Cohen, L., et al. (2014). Can the meaning of multiple words be integrated unconsciously? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1641), 20130212.Google Scholar
  38. Van Opstal, F., Calderon, C. B., Gevers, W., & Verguts, T. (2011). Setting the stage subliminally: unconscious context effects. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1860–1864.Google Scholar
  39. Van Opstal, F., Gevers, W., Osman, M., & Verguts, T. (2010). Unconscious task application. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(4), 999–1006.Google Scholar
  40. Wang, D., Tang, L., Liang, Q., Jou, J., Ma, Y., Pan, W., et al. (2017). Unconscious integration of pointing relation between two masked arrows. Visual Cognition, 25(9–10), 928–939.Google Scholar
  41. Wang, Y., Wang, Y., Liu, P., Dai, D., Di, M., & Chen, Q. (2016). The availability of attentional resources modulates the inhibitory strength related to weakly activated priming. Atten Percept Psychophys, 78(6), 1655–1664.Google Scholar
  42. Yun, X., Li, W., Qiu, J., Jou, J., Wei, D., Tu, S., et al. (2011). Neural mechanisms of subliminal priming for traumatic episodic memory: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 498(1), 10–14.Google Scholar
  43. Zhou, J., Lee, C., Li, K., Tien, Y., & Yeh, S. (2016). Does temporal integration occur for unrecognizable words in visual crowding? PloS One, 11(2), e149355.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, School of Public AdministrationGuizhou University of Finance and EconomicsGuiyangChina
  2. 2.The Center of College Students’Psychological DevelopmentSouthwest Medical UniversityLuzhouChina
  3. 3.Department of Psychology, Institute of EducationChina West Normal UniversityNanchongChina
  4. 4.Department of Psychological ScienceUniversity of Texas – Rio Grande ValleyEdinburgUSA

Personalised recommendations