Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 47, Issue 4, pp 947–957 | Cite as

Phrase-Final Words in Greek Storytelling Speech: A Study on the Effect of a Culturally-Specific Prosodic Feature on Short-Term Memory

  • Ariadne Loutrari
  • Freideriki Tselekidou
  • Hariklia Proios
Article
  • 57 Downloads

Abstract

Prosodic patterns of speech appear to make a critical contribution to memory-related processing. We considered the case of a previously unexplored prosodic feature of Greek storytelling and its effect on free recall in thirty typically developing children between the ages of 10 and 12 years, using short ecologically valid auditory stimuli. The combination of a falling pitch contour and, more notably, extensive final-syllable vowel lengthening, which gives rise to the prosodic feature in question, led to statistically significantly higher performance in comparison to neutral phrase-final prosody. Number of syllables in target words did not reveal substantial difference in performance. The current study presents a previously undocumented culturally-specific prosodic pattern and its effect on short-term memory.

Keywords

Prosody Storytelling Short-term memory Pitch Duration 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alm, C. O., & Sproat, R. (2005). Perceptions of emotions in expressive storytelling. In INTERSPEECH (pp. 533–536).Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, A., Lin, C. Y., & Wang, M. (2013). Native and novel language prosodic sensitivity in english-speaking children with and without Dyslexia. Dyslexia, 19, 92–112.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Arvaniti, A. (1999). Standard modern Greek. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 29, 167–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baltazani, M. (2007). Prosodic rhythm and the status of vowel reduction in Greek. Selected Papers on Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, 17, 31–43.Google Scholar
  6. Brierley, B., Medford, N., Shaw, P., & David, A. S. (2007). Emotional memory for words: Separating content and context. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 495–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Campoy, G. (2011). Retroactive interference in short-term memory and the word-length effect. Acta Psychologica, 138, 135–142.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cowan, N., Day, L., Saults, J. S., Keller, T. A., Johnson, T., & Flores, L. (1992). The role of verbal output time in the effects of word length on immediate memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cutler, A., Dahan, D., & Van Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature review. Language and Speech, 40, 141–201.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Doukhan, D., Rilliard, A., Rosset, S., Adda-Decker, M., & d’Alessandro, C. (2011). Prosodic analysis of a corpus of tales. In INTERSPEECH (pp. 3129–3132).Google Scholar
  11. Fairfield, B., Di Domenico, A., Serricchio, S., Borella, E., & Mammarella., N. (2016). Emotional prosody effects on verbal memory in older and younger adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1219690.Google Scholar
  12. Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C. (1991). Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology, 27, 209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gathercole, S. E., Service, E., Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., & Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 65–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gilbert, A. C., & Boucher, V. J. (2007). What do listeners attend to in hearing prosodic structures? Investigating the human speech-parser using short-term recall. In INTERSPEECH (pp. 430–433).Google Scholar
  15. Goldman, S. R., Meyerson, P. M., & Cote, N. (2006). Poetry as a mnemonic prompt in children’s stories. Reading Psychology, 27, 345–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grandjean, D., Bänziger, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2006). Intonation as an interface between language and affect. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 235–247.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words. Journal of Child Language, 34, 677–687.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Jalbert, A., Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2011). Does length or neighborhood size cause the word length effect? Memory & Cognition, 39, 1198–1210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (1999). The effects of emotions on voice quality. In Proceedings of the XIVth international congress of phonetic sciences (pp. 2029–2032). San Francisco: University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
  20. Katkov, M., Romani, S., & Tsodyks, M. (2014). Word length effect in free recall of randomly assembled word lists. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 8, 129.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Lindfield, C. K., Wingfield, A., & Goodglass, H. (1999). The role of prosody in the mental lexicon. Brain and Language, 68, 312–317.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Männel, C., Schaadt, G., Illner, F. K., van der Meer, E., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). Phonological abilities in literacy-impaired children: Brain potentials reveal deficient phoneme discrimination, but intact prosodic processing. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 14–25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Marshall, C. R., Harcourt-Brown, S., Ramus, F., & Van Der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). The link between prosody and language skills in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and/or dyslexia. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44, 466–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Michas, I. C., & Henry, L. A. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 147–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mira, W. A., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2013). The impact of reading expressiveness on the listening comprehension of storybooks by prekindergarten children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 44, 183–194.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Montaño, R., & Alías, F. (2016). The role of prosody and voice quality in indirect storytelling speech: Annotation methodology and expressive categories. Speech Communication, 85, 8–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nieuwenhuis-Mark, R. E., Schalk, K., & de Graaf, N. (2009). Free recall and learning of emotional word lists in very elderly people with and without dementia. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 24, 155–162.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Reeves, C., Schmauder, R. A., & Morris, K. R. (2000). Stress grouping improves performance on an immediate serial list recall task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1638–1654.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Sakkalou, E., & Gattis, M. (2012). Infants infer intentions from prosody. Cognitive Development, 27, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sarkar, P., Haque, A., Dutta, A. K., Reddy, G., Harikrishna, D. M., Dhara, P. et al. (2014). Designing prosody rule-set for converting neutral TTS speech to storytelling style speech for indian languages: Bengali, Hindi and Telugu. In Contemporary computing (IC3), 2014 seventh international conference (pp. 473–477).Google Scholar
  31. Savino, M., Bosco, A., & Grice, M. (2013). Intonation and positional effects in spoken serial recall. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 35, 3360–3365.Google Scholar
  32. Sober, J. D., VanWormer, L. A., & Arruda, J. E. (2016). Age-related differences in recall for words using semantics and prosody. The Journal of General Psychology, 143, 67–77.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Theune, M., Meijs, K., Heylen, D., & Ordelman, R. (2006). Generating expressive speech for storytelling applications. IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech and Language Processing, 14, 1137–1144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Thompson, W. F., Marin, M. M., & Stewart, L. (2012). Reduced sensitivity to emotional prosody in congenital amusia rekindles the musical protolanguage hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy Sciences USA, 109, 19027–19032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 65–82.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Wiethoff, S., Wildgruber, D., Kreifelts, B., Becker, H., Herbert, C., Grodd, W., et al. (2008). Cerebral processing of emotional prosody—Influence of acoustic parameters and arousal. Neuroimage, 39, 885–893.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ariadne Loutrari
    • 1
  • Freideriki Tselekidou
    • 2
  • Hariklia Proios
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Applied Linguistics and CommunicationBirkbeck, University of LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.Department of Education and Social PolicyUniversity of MacedoniaThessalonikiGreece

Personalised recommendations