Altarriba, J., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 31(4), 578–602.
Article
Google Scholar
Altarriba, J., & Bauer, L. M. (2004). The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: A comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. American Journal of Psychology, 117, 389–410.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Andersen, E. S., Dunlea, A., & Kekelis, L. (1984). Blind children’s language: Resolving some differences. Journal of Child Language, 11(3), 645–664.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ansorge, U., & Leder, H. (2017). Wahrnehmung und Aufmerksamkeit (2nd ed.). Wiesbaden: Springer.
Book
Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.
Barsalou, L. W., & Wiemer-Hastings, K. (2005). Situating abstract concepts. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought (pp. 129–163). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Baschek, I.-L., Bredenkamp, J., Oehrle, B., & Wippich, W. (1977). Bestimmung der Bildhaftigkeit (I), Konkretheit (C) und der Bedeutungshaltigkeit (m’) von 800 Substantiven. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, XXIV(3), 353–396.
Google Scholar
Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.
Article
Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKierman, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 905–917.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bleasdale, F. A. (1987). Concreteness-dependent associative priming: Separate lexical organization for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 582–594.
Google Scholar
Breedin, S. D., Saffran, E. M., & Coslett, H. B. (1994). Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with sementic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 617–660.
Article
Google Scholar
Brown, G. D. A., & Watson, F. L. (1987). First in, first out: Word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 203–216.
Article
Google Scholar
Büchel, C., Price, C. J., & Friston, K. (1998). A multimodal language region in the ventral visual pathway. Nature, 394, 274–277.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Coltheart, V., Laxon, V. J., & Keating, C. (1988). Effects of word imageability and age of acquisition on children’s reading. British Journal of Psychology Society, 79, 1–12.
Article
Google Scholar
De Bleser, R. (2006). A linguist’s view on progressive anomia: Evidence for delbrück (1886) in modern neurolinguistic research. Cortex, 42, 805–810.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association. Journal Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15(5), 824–845.
Google Scholar
Duden, (2006). Die deutsche Grammatik (4th ed.). Mannheim: Dudenverlag.
Google Scholar
Dunlea, A. (1989). Vision and the emergence of meaning: Blind and sighted children’s early language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Eaglemann, D. M., Kagan, A. D., Nelson, S. S., Sagaram, D., & Sarma, A. K. (2007). A standardized test battery for the study of Synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 159(1), 139–145.
Article
Google Scholar
Ewald, P. (1992). Konkreta versus Abstrakta. Zur semantischen Subklassifikation deutscher Substantive. Sprachwissenschaft, 17, 259–281.
Google Scholar
Franklin, S. (1989). Dissociations in auditory word comprehension: Evidence from nine fluent aphasic patients. Aphasiology, 3(3), 189–207.
Article
Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of incosistent interactions between lexical amiliarity and Orothography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 256–281.
Article
Google Scholar
Goh, W. D., Yap, M. J., Lau, M. C., NG, M. M. R., & Tan, L. C. (2016). Semantic richness effects in spoken word recognition: A lexical decision and semantic categorization megastudy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–10.
Article
Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1986). Phänomenologie der Lebenswelt. Ausgewählte Texte I. Dietzingen: Reclam.
Google Scholar
Howell, J. R., & Bryden, M. P. (1987). The effects of word orientation and imageability on visual half-field presentations with a lexical decision task. Neuropsychologia, 25(3), 527–538.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kousta, S.-T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 14–34.
Article
Google Scholar
Lee, C., & Federmeier, K. (2008). To watch, to see, and to differ. An event-related potential study of concreteness effects as a function of word class and lexical ambiguity. Brain and Language, 104(2), 145–158.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mills, C. B., Edelson, S. K., Thomas, A. T., Simon-Dack, S. L., & Innis, J. A. (2002). The color of two alphabets for a multilingual synesthete. Perception, 31, 1371–1394.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mills, C. B., Innis, J., Westendorf, T., Owsianiecki, L., & McDonald, A. (2006). Effect of a synesthete’s photisms on name recall. Cortex, 42, 155–163.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.
Article
Google Scholar
Noppeney, U., & Price, C. J. (2004). Retrieval of abstract semantic. NeuroImage, 22, 164–170.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1968). A factor-analytic study of word attributes and verbal learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 7, 41–49.
Article
Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual-coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255–287.
Article
Google Scholar
Paivio, A., & Begg, I. (1981). Imagery and comprehension latencies as a function of sentence concreteness and structure. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 408–412.
Article
Google Scholar
Paivio, A., & Okovita, H. W. (1971). Word imagery modalities and associative learning in blind and sighted subjects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 506–510.
Article
Google Scholar
Papagno, C., Fogliata, A., Catricalà, E., & Miniussi, C. (2009). The lexical processing of abstract and concrete nouns. Brain research, 1263, 78–86.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (2014). Contrasting views on the pragmatic abilities of blind children. Enfance, 1, 73–88.
Article
Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M., & Castro, J. (1992). Pragmatic functions of blind and sighted children’s language: A twin case study. First Language, 12, 17–37.
Article
Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. https://www.R-project.org/
Reilly, J., Peele, J. E., & Grossman, M. (2007). A unitary semantics account of reverse concreteness effects in semantic dementia. Brain and Language, 103, 248–249.
Article
Google Scholar
Röder, B., & Rösler, F. (2004). Kompensatorische Plastizität bei blinden Menschen. Was Blinde über die Adaptivität des Gehirns verraten. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 15(4), 243–264.
Article
Google Scholar
Samson, D., & Pillon, A. (2003). Concreteness effects in lexical tasks. Access to a mental image? Brain and Language, 87, 25–26.
Article
Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1991). Why are abstract concepts hard to understand. In P. J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 223–250). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Akin, C. E. (1993). Developmental trends in lexical decision for abstract and concrete words. Reading Research Report, 1, 1–18.
Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., Harnishfeger, K. K., & Stowe, R. W. (1988). Context availability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 499–520.
Article
Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Shoben, E. J. (1983). Differential context effects in the comprehension of abstract and concrete verbal materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 9(1), 82–102.
Google Scholar
Snefjella, B., & Kuperman, V. (2016). It’s all in the delivery: Effects of context valence, arousal, and concreteness on visual word processing. Cognition, 156, 135–146.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S.-T., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., & Devlin, J. T., et al. (2013). The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 1767–1777.
Warrington, E., & Shallice, T. (1984). Categoric specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–853.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wiemer-Hastings, K., Krug, J., & Xu, X. (2001). Imagery, context availability, contextual constraint, and abstractness. In: Paper presented at the 23rd Annual Conference Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah.
Xiao, X., Zhao, D., Zhang, Q., & Guo, C. (2012). Retrieval of concrete words involves more conctextual information than abstract words: Multiple components for the concreteness effect. Brain & Language, 120, 251–258.
Article
Google Scholar