Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 46, Issue 2, pp 481–496 | Cite as

Gender Differences in Being Thanked for Performing a Favor

  • Albert N. KatzEmail author
  • Jonathan A. R. Woodbury
Article

Abstract

We examined gender differences in how one responds to being thanked for a favor. Using experimental passages, we manipulated who requested the favor and the manner in which the favor is asked. Male and female participants received a set of scenarios in which social status, gender and directness of the request were orthogonally varied. Although male and female participants had very similar perceptions of whether the favor was a command or not, male but not female participants, generated more accommodating and fewer non-accommodating acknowledgments when thanked. The effect was strongest when the request was made by a boss (relative to a peer), especially if the boss was male, and made the request in a direct manner. These data are consistent with the notion that, for males, more than so for females, interactions that make salient one’s dominance status is relevant to gender-identity, and is linguistically reflected in a basic social exchange.

Keywords

Social exchange language Gendered language Social hierarchies Expressions of gratitude acknowledgment 

References

  1. Cameron, D. (2005). Language, gender and sexuality: Current issues and new directions. Applied Linguistics, 26, 482–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Colston, H. (2002). Pragmatic justifications for non-literal gratitude acknowledgments: “Oh sure, anytime”. Metaphor and Symbol, 17, 205–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Eisenstein, M., & Bodman, J. (1986). “I very much appreciate”: Expressions of gratitude by native and non-native speakers of American English. Applied Linguistics, 7, 167–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Fournier, M., Moskowitz, D., & Zuroff, D. (2002). Social rank strategies in hierarchical relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 425–433.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Goldschmidt, M. M. (1998). Do me a favor: A descriptive analysis of favor asking sequences in American English. Journal of Pragmatics, 29, 129–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Holtgraves, T. (1986). Language structure in social interaction: Perceptions of direct and indirect speech acts and interactants who use them. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 305–314.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Holtgraves, T. (1997). Politeness and memory for the wording of remarks. Memory and Cognition, 25, 106–116.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Hussey, K. A., & Katz, A. N. (2006). Metaphor production in online conversation: Gender and friendship status. Discourse Processes, 42, 75–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hussey, K. A., Katz, A. N., & Leith, S. A. (2015). Gendered language in interactive discourse. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44, 417–433.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Katz, A., Lenhardt, M., & Mitchell, K. (2007). On acknowledging thanks for performing a favor. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 233–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Kampf, Z. (2009). Public (non-) apologies: The discourse of minimizing responsibility. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 2257–2270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Kampf, Z. (2016). All the best! Performing solidarity in political discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 93, 47–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Maltz, D. J., & Borker, R. A. (1982). A cultural approach to male–female miscommunication. In J. J. Gumpertz (Ed.), Language and social identity (pp. 196–216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Mulac, A., Bradac, J. J., & Gibbons, P. (2001). Empirical support for gender-as-culture hypothesis: An intercultural analysis of male/female language differences. Human Communication Research, 27, 121–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Nattinger, J., & DeCarrico, J. (1992). Lexical phrases and language teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Newman, M., Groom, C., Handelman, L., & Pennebaker, J. (2008). Gender differences in language use: An analysis of 14,000 text samples. Discourse Processes, 45, 211–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Palomares, N. (2009). Women are sort of more tentative then men, aren’t they? Communication Research, 36, 538–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Schauer, G., & Adolphs, S. (2006). Expressions of gratitude in corpus and DCT data: Vocabulary, formulaic sequences, and pedagogy. System, 34(1), 119–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Slamacka, N., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604.Google Scholar
  20. Symons, C., & Johnson, B. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–394.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand: Women and men in conversation. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
  22. Wray, A. (2000). Formulaic sequences in second language teaching: Principles and practice. Applied Linguistics, 21, 463–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Western OntarioLondonCanada
  2. 2.PetFocus Woodbury Veterinary HospitalHaliaxCanada

Personalised recommendations