Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 46, Issue 2, pp 457–468 | Cite as

Are Vocal Pitch Changes in Response to Facial Expressions of Emotions Potential Cues of Empathy? A Preliminary Report

  • Sethu KarthikeyanEmail author
  • Vijayachandra Ramachandra
Article

Abstract

Previous research has demonstrated that even brief exposures to facial expressions of emotions elicit facial mimicry in receivers in the form of corresponding facial muscle movements. As well, vocal and verbal patterns of speakers converge in conversations, a type of vocal mimicry. There is also evidence of cross-modal mimicry in which emotional vocalizations elicit corresponding facial muscle activity. Further, empathic capacity has been associated with enhanced tendency towards facial mimicry as well as verbal synchrony. We investigated a type of potential cross-modal mimicry in a simulated dyadic situation. Specifically, we examined the influence of facial expressions of happy, sad, and neutral emotions on the vocal pitch of receivers, and its potential association with empathy. Results indicated that whereas both mean pitch and variability of pitch varied somewhat in the predicted directions, empathy was correlated with the difference in the variability of pitch while speaking to the sad and neutral faces. Discussion of results considers the dimensional nature of emotional vocalizations and possible future directions.

Keywords

Facial emotions Vocal pitch Cross-modal mimicry Empathy Arousal and valence 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Christina Dixon, Karen Buckley, and Sydney Pauline for assistance with data collection.

References

  1. Aziz-Zadeh, L., Sheng, T., & Gheytanchi, A. (2010). Common premotor regions for the perception and production of prosody and correlations with empathy and prosodic ability. PLoS One, 5, e8759.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Bachorowski, J.-A. (1999). Vocal expression and perception of emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 53–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dimberg, U., Andréasson, P., & Thunberg, M. (2011). Emotional empathy and facial reactions to facial expressions of emotions. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25, 26–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (2005). What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Eriksson, A., & Wretling, P. (1997). How flexible is the human voice? A case study of mimicry. In Proceedings of the european conference of speech technology, Rhodes, Greece (pp. 1043–1046).Google Scholar
  11. Fraccaro, P. J., Jones, B. C., Vukovic, J., Smith, F. G., Watkins, C. D., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2011). Experimental evidence that women speak in a higher voice pitch to men they find attractive. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 9, 57–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  13. Grossman, R. B., Edelson, L. R., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2013). Emotional facial and vocal expressions during story retelling by children and adolescents with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 1035–1044.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Grossman, R. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2012). “Who said that?” Matching of low- and high-intensity emotional prosody to facial expressions by adolescents with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2546–2557.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Gueguen, N., Jacob, C., & Martin, A. (2009). Mimicry in social interaction: Its effect on human judgment and behavior. European Journal of Social Sciences, 8, 253–259.Google Scholar
  16. Hauser, D. J., Preston, S. D., & Stansfield, R. B. (2014). Altruism in the wild: When affiliative motives to help positive people overtake empathic motives to help the distressed. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 143, 1295–1305.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hawk, S. T., Fischer, A. H., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Face the noise: Embodied responses to nonverbal vocalizations of discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 796–814.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hess, U., Houde, S., & Fischer, A. (2014). Do we mimic what we see or what we know? In C. von Scheve & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Hietanen, J. R., Surakka, V., & Linnenkoski, I. (1998). Facial electromyographic responses to vocal affect expressions. Psychophysiology, 35, 530–536.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Hsee, C. K., Hatfield, C., Carlson, J. G., & Chemtob, C. (1990). The effect of power on susceptibility to emotional contagion. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 327–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hughes, S. M., Farley, S. D., & Rhodes, B. C. (2010). Vocal and physiological changes in response to the physical attractiveness of conversational partners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34, 155–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Imel, Z. E., Barco, J. S., Brown, H. J., Baucom, B. R., Kircher, J. C., Baer, J. S., et al. (2014). The association of therapist empathy and synchrony in vocally encoded arousal. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61, 146–153.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Karthikeyan, S., & Locke, J. L. (2015). Men’s evaluation of women’s speech in a simulated dating context: Effects of female fertility on vocal pitch and attractiveness. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 9, 55–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Karthikeyan, S., Rammairone, B., & Ramachandra, V. (2016). The bouba-kiki phenomenon tested via schematic drawings of facial expressions: Further validation of the internal simulation hypothesis. i-Perception, 7, 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Laukka, P., Juslin, P. N., & Bresin, R. (2005). A dimensional approach to vocal expression of emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 633–653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lawrence, L., & Abdel-Nabi, D. (2011). The BARTA (University of Bolton Affect Recognition Tri-Stimulus Approach): “A systematic journey from emoticons to humans via toontown”. In Paper presented at the BPS Annual Conference, Glasgow, UK.Google Scholar
  28. Lehane, C. M. (2015). Male and female differences in nonconscious mimicry: A systematic review. Journal of European Psychology Students, 6, 34–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Magnee, M. J. C. M., Stekelenburg, J. J., Kemner, C., & de Gelder, B. (2006). Similar facial electromyographic responses to faces, voices, and body expressions. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 18, 369–372.Google Scholar
  30. Manson, J. H., Bryant, G. A., Gervais, M. M., & Kline, M. A. (2013). Convergence of speech rate in conversation predicts cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 419–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2009). Reflections on the mirror neuron system: Their evolutionary functions beyond motor representation. In J. A. Pineda (Ed.), Mirror neuron systems (pp. 39–62). New York: Humana Press.Google Scholar
  32. Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage, 39, 2076–2085.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Pickering, M., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 1–57.Google Scholar
  34. Pisanski, K., Cartei, V., McGettigan, C., Raine, J., & Reby, D. (2016). Voice modulation: A window into the origins of human vocal control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 304–318.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Provine, R. R. (1992). Contagious laughter: Laughter is a sufficient stimulus for laughter and smiles. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ramachandra, V. (2009). On whether mirror neurons play a significant role in processing affective prosody. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108, 30–36.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Ramachandra, V., Depalma, N., & Lisiewski, S. (2009). The role of mirror neurons in processing vocal emotions: Evidence from psychophysiological data. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119, 681–690.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Russel, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 116, 223–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J., & Fernandez-Dols, J. (2003). Facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 329–349.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Sato, W., Fujimura, T., Kochiyama, T., & Suzuki, N. (2013). Relationships among facial mimicry, emotional experience, and emotion recognition. PLoS One, 8, e57889.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. G. (2001). Emotion inferences from vocal expression correlate across languages and cultures. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 32, 76–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Seibt, B., Mühlberger, A., Likowski, K. U., & Weyers, P. (2015). Facial mimicry in its social setting. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1122.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 14, 71–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 393–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Willis, M. L., Palermo, R., & Burke, D. (2011). Judging approachability on the face of it: The influence of face and body expressions on the perception of approachability. Emotion, 11, 514–523.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Zraick, R. I., Gentry, M. A., Smith-Olinde, L., & Gregg, B. A. (2006). The effect of speaking context on elicitation of habitual pitch. Journal of Voice, 20, 545–554.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Communication Sciences and Disorders Program, College of Health ProfessionsPace UniversityNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.Language and Cognition Lab, Department of Communication Sciences and DisordersMarywood UniversityScrantonUSA

Personalised recommendations