Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 46, Issue 2, pp 329–343 | Cite as

The Influence of Child-Directed Speech on Word Learning and Comprehension

  • Cassandra Foursha-StevensonEmail author
  • Taylor Schembri
  • Elena Nicoladis
  • Cody Eriksen
Article

Abstract

This paper describes an investigation into the function of child-directed speech (CDS) across development. In the first experiment, 10–21-month-olds were presented with familiar words in CDS and trained on novel words in CDS or adult-directed speech (ADS). All children preferred the matching display for familiar words. However, only older toddlers in the CDS condition preferred the matching display for novel words. In Experiment 2, children 3–6 years of age were presented with a sentence comprehension task in CDS or ADS. Older children performed better overall than younger children with 5- and 6-year-olds performing above chance regardless of speech condition, while 3- and 4-year-olds only performed above chance when the sentences were presented in CDS. These findings provide support for the theory that CDS is most effective at the beginning of acquisition for particular constructions (e.g. vocabulary acquisition, syntactic comprehension) rather than at a particular age or for a particular task.

Keywords

Child-directed speech Word learning Sentence comprehension 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by a Mount Royal University Internal Research Grant.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Andruski, J. E., Kuhl, P. K., & Hayashi, A. (1999). Point vowels in Japanese mothers’ speech to infants and adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(2), 1095–1096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Benasich, A. A., & Tallal, P. (2002). Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behavioural Brain Research, 136, 31–49.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  4. Braarud, H. C., & Stormark, K. M. (2008). Prosodic modification and vocal adjustments in mother‘s speech during face-to-face interaction with their two- to four-month-old infants: A double video study. Social Development, 17(4), 1074–1084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Broesch, T. L., & Bryant, G. A. (2015). Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(1), 31–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown, A., & Draper, P. (2003). Accommodative speech and terms of endearment: Elements of a language mode often experienced by older adults. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 41, 15–21.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Byers-Heinlein, K., Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2013). The development of associative learning in monolingual and bilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 198–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chan, A., Meints, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children’s comprehension of English SVO word order revisited: Testing the same children in act-out and intermodal preferential looking tasks. Cognitive Development, 25, 30–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20(4), 477–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65, 1663–1677.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. de Boer, B., & Kuhl, P. K. (2003). Investigating the role of infant-directed speech with a computer model. Acoustics Research Letters Online, 4, 129–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Englund, K. (2006). Changes in infant directed speech in the first six months. Infant and Child Development, 15(2), 139–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Espy, K. A., Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (2004). Development of auditory event-related potentials in young children and relations to word-level reading abilities at age 8 years. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 9–38.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(2), 181–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behaviour and Development, 10, 279–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Golinkoff, R. M., & Alioto, A. (1995). Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: Implications for language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 22, 703–726.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Golinkoff, R. M., Ma, W., Song, L., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2013). Twenty five years using the intermodal preferential looking paradigm to study language acquisition: What have we learned? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 316–339.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2015). (Baby) talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(5), 339–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., & Shimpi, P. (2004). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 182–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kemler Nelson, D. G., & Smith, J. D. (1989). Analytic and holistic processing in reflection-impulsivity and cognitive development. In T. Globerson & T. Zelniker (Eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 116–140). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  21. Kempe, V., & Brooks, P. J. (2005). The role of diminutives in the acquisition of Russian gender: Can elements of child-directed speech aid in learning morphology? Language Learning, 55(S1), 139–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kempe, V., Brooks, P. J., Mironova, N., Pershukova, A., & Fedorova, O. (2007). Playing with word endings: Morphological variation in the learning of Russian noun infections. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 55–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kempe, V., Schaeffler, S., & Thoresen, J. C. (2010). Prosodic disambiguation in child-directed speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 204–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., et al. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277(5326), 684–686.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Lidz, J., Waxman, S., & Freedman, J. (2003). What infants know about syntax, but couldn’t have leaned: Experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months. Cognition, 89, B65–B73.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Liu, H., Kuhl, P., & Tsao, F. (2003). An association between mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science, 6(3), F1–F10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2011). Word learning in infant-and adult-directed speech. Language Learning and Development, 7(3), 185–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Martin, A., Schatz, T., Versteegh, M., Miyazawa, K., Mazuka, R., Dupoux, E., et al. (2015). Mothers speak less clearly to infants than to adults: A comprehensive test of the hyperarticulation hypothesis. Psychological Science, 26(3), 341–347.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Matychuk, P. (2005). The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: A case study. Language Sciences, 27(3), 301–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. McRoberts, G. W., McDonough, C., & Lakusta, L. (2009). The role of verbal repetition in the development of infant speech preferences from 4 to 14 months of age. Infancy, 14(2), 162–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Molfese, D. L. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72(3), 238–245.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6, 557–567.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Segal, J., & Newman, R. S. (2015). Infant preferences for structural and prosodic properties of infant-directed speech in the second year of life. Infancy, 20(3), 339–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(5), 1–38.Google Scholar
  35. Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & Best, C. T. (2002). Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk? Infancy, 3(3), 365–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Singh, L., Nestor, S., Parikh, C., & Yull, A. (2009). Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy, 14(6), 654–666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Snow, C. E. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: From input to interaction. In C. Snow & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31–49). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy, 7(1), 53–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74(3), 209–253.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Wagner, L., Swensen, L. D., & Naigles, L. R. (2009). Children’s early productivity with verbal morphology. Cognitive Development, 24, 223–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Yuan, S., & Fisher, C. (2009). Really? She blicked the baby? Two-year-olds learn combinatorial facts about verbs by listening. Psychological Science, 20(5), 619–626.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Zangl, R., & Mills, D. L. (2007). Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. Infancy, 11(1), 31–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cassandra Foursha-Stevenson
    • 1
    Email author
  • Taylor Schembri
    • 2
  • Elena Nicoladis
    • 3
  • Cody Eriksen
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyMount Royal UniversityCalgaryCanada
  2. 2.University of CalgaryCalgaryCanada
  3. 3.University of AlbertaEdmontonCanada
  4. 4.Yorkville UniversityFrederictonCanada

Personalised recommendations