Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 45, Issue 6, pp 1473–1497 | Cite as

Their Hands Have Lost Their Bones: Exploring Cultural Scripts in Two West African Affect Lexica

  • Vivian Dzokoto
  • Nicole SenftEmail author
  • Lily Kpobi
  • Princess-Melissa Washington-Nortey
Article

Abstract

Previous research has indicated the importance of embodiment in West African emotion lexica. The current study aims to explore the pervasiveness of this cultural script through the analysis of the emotional lexica of two West African languages (Ga and Ewe) from Southern Ghana that have been featured minimally in previous emotion research. The analysis indicated that embodiment was an important cultural script in both affective lexica. However, interpersonal representations of emotions were also present. Further, emotion words in the two languages differed in the more specific loci of emotions.

Keywords

Affect West Africa Somatization 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge Dzifa Attah and John Dotse for their help with data collection, and Adote Anum, Joseph Adonu and Mrs Tetteh for translation assistance.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Abu-Lughod, L., & Lutz, C. A. (1990). Introduction: Emotion, discourse, and the politics of everyday life. Language and the Politics of Emotion, 1, 1–23.Google Scholar
  2. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Akrofi, C. A., Takyi, B. K., & Botchey, G. L. (1996). An English-Akan-Ga-Ewe dictionary. Accra: Waterville Publishing House.Google Scholar
  4. Alonso-Arbiol, I., Shaver, P. R., Fraley, R. C., Oronoz, B., Unzurrunzaga, E., & Urizar, R. (2006). Structure of the Basque emotion lexicon. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 836–865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ameka, F. K. (2002). Cultural scripting of body parts for emotions: On ‘jealousy’ and related emotions in Ewe. Pragmatics & Cognition, 10, 27–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baumann, J. F., Edwards, E. C., Font, G., Tereshinski, C. A., Kame’enui, E. J., & Olejnik, S. (2002). Teaching morphemic and contextual analysis to fifth-grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 37, 150–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Betancourt, T. S., Frounfelker, R., Mishra, T., Hussein, A., & Falzarano, R. (2015). Addressing health disparities in the mental health of refugee children and adolescents through community-based participatory research: A study in two communities. American Journal Of Public Health, 105, S475–S482. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302504.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Bendor-Samuel, J. (Ed.). (1989). The Niger-Congo Languages: A Classification and Description of Africa’s Largest Language Family. Lanham: University Press of America.Google Scholar
  9. Bhumiratana, N., Adhikari, K., & Chambers, E. (2014). The development of an emotion lexicon for the coffee drinking experience. Food Research International, 61, 83–6192. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2014.03.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bodomo, A. B. (1994). Language, history and culture in northern Ghana: An introduction to the Mabia linguistic group. Nordic Journal of African Studies, 3, 25–43.Google Scholar
  11. Bodomo, A. B. (1996). On language and development in Africa: The case of Ghana. Nordic Journal of African Studies, 5, 31–51.Google Scholar
  12. Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2012). The construction of emotion in interactions, relationships, and cultures. Emotion Review, 4, 221–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  14. Briggs, J. L. (1970). Never in anger: Portrait of an Eskimo family (Vol. 12). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Chentsova-Dutton, Y., & Dzokoto, V. (2014). Listen to your heart: The cultural shaping of interoceptive awareness and accuracy. Emotion, 14, 666–678.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., Senft, N., & Ryder, A. G. (2014). Listening to negative emotions: How culture constrains what we hear. In G. Parrott (Ed.), The positive side of negative emotions. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  17. Choi, E. (2014). Distress communication in cultural context: Examination of Koreans and Americans. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgetown University, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  18. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 91–115). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
  19. Cosser, M. (1998). The deployment of code in Soyinka’s the road: A stylistic analysis. In E. Epstein & R. Kole (Eds.), The language of African literature (pp. 113–152). Trenton, NJ: Africa World Press.Google Scholar
  20. Csordas, T. J. (1993). Somatic modes of attention. Cultural Anthropology, 8, 135–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Codjoe, S. N. A. & Amoah, S (2013). Population size, composition, and age-sex structure. In Ghana Statistical Service (2013). 2010 Population & Housing Census: National Analytical Report (pp. 50–66).Google Scholar
  22. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.44.1.113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Doi, T. (1981). The anatomy of dependence. Tokyo: Kodansha International.Google Scholar
  24. Dzokoto, V. A., & Okazaki, S. (2006). Happiness in the eye and the heart: Somatic referencing in West African Emotion Lexicon. Journal of Black Psychology, 32, 117–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dzokoto, V. (2010). Different ways of feeling: Emotion and somatic awareness in Ghanaians and Euro-Americans. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 4, 68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Dzokoto, V., Opare-Henaku, A., & Kpobi, L. (2013). Somatic referencing and psychologisation in emotion narratives: A USA–Ghana comparison. Psychology and Developing Societies, 25, 311–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for research and an integration of findings. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  28. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Miller, P., Carlo, G., Poulin, R., et al. (1991). Personality and socialization correlates of vicarious emotional responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 459–470.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Enfield, N. J., & Wierzbicka, A. (2002). The body in description of emotion. Pragmatics & Cognition, 10, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Feldman-Barrett, L., Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2007). Language as context for the perception of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 327–332. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Fetterman, D. (2010). Ethnography: Step-by-step. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  32. Frijda, N. H., Markam, S., Sato, K., & Wiers, R. (1995). Emotion and emotion words. In J. A. Russell, J. M. Fernández-Dols, A. S. R. Manstead, & J. C. Wellenkamp (Eds.), Everyday conceptions of emotion (pp. 121–143). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fugate, J. M. B., Gouzoules, H., & Feldman-Barrett, L. (2010). Reading chimpanzee faces: Evidence for the role of verbal labels in categorical perception of emotion. Emotion, 10, 544–554. doi: 10.1037/a0019017.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Gendron, M., Lindquist, K. A., Barsalou, L., & Feldman-Barrett, L. (2012). Emotion words shape emotion percepts. Emotion, 12, 314–325. doi: 10.1037/a0026007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Gerber, E. R. (1985). Rage and obligation: Samoan emotion in conflict. In G. M. White & J. Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Person, self, and experience: exploring pacific ethnopsychologies (pp. 121–167). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  36. Gertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  37. Geurts, K. L. (2003). Culture and the senses: Bodily ways of knowing in an African community (Vol. 3). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ghana Statistical Service (2013). 2010 population and housing census national analytical report.Google Scholar
  39. Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2005). Language and thought. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Goddard, C. (1996). The “social emotions” of Malay (Bahasa melayu). Ethos, 24, 426–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Goddard, C., & Wierzbicka, A. (2004). Cultural scripts: What are they and what are they good for. Intercultural Pragmatics, 1, 153–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Goswami, S., & Raju, S. (2011). Are tonal and non-tonal languages lateralized differently in bilingual tonal language speakers? Language in India, 11, 12–21.Google Scholar
  43. Gyekye, K. (1996). African cultural values: An introduction. Philadelphia, PA: Sankofa.Google Scholar
  44. Harkins, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (2001). Emotions in crosslinguistic perspective. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hinton, D., & Hinton, S. (2002). Panic disorder, somatization, and the new cross-cultural psychiatry: The seven bodies of a medical anthropology of panic. Culture, Medicine & Psychiatry, 26, 155–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Ho, J. W. Y. (2009). The language of anger in Chinese and English narratives. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13, 481–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Holland, D., & Quinn, N. (1987). Cultural models in language and thought (Vol. xii). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Hupka, R. B., Zaleski, Z., Otto, J., Reidl, L., & Tarabrina, N. V. (1996). Anger, envy, fear, and jealousy as felt in the body: A five-nation study. Cross-Cultural Research, 30, 243–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Keith, P. (2013). The benfits of speaking tonal languages. Teaching Music, 21, 8–18.Google Scholar
  50. Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2001). Social functions of emotions. In T. J. Mayne & G. A. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 192–213). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  51. Kenstowicz, M. (2003). The role of perception in loanword phonology. Studies in African Linguistics, 32, 95–112.Google Scholar
  52. Kim-Prieto, C., & Eid, M. (2004). Norms for experiencing emotions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5, 241–268. doi: 10.1007/s10531-004-8787-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345–379. doi: 10.1007/BF00992553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Kövecses, Z. (1990). Emotion concepts. New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Kövecses, Z. (2000). The scope of metaphor. In A. Barcelona (Ed.), Metaphor and metonymy at the crossroads: A cognitive perspective (pp. 30–79). New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  56. Kropp-Dakubu, M. E. (1988). The languages of Ghana. London: Associated Book Publishers.Google Scholar
  57. Kropp-Dakubu, M. E. (1999). Ga-English dictionary with English-Ga Index. Accra, Ghana: Black Mask Ltd.Google Scholar
  58. Lawson, R. (2014). ’Don’t even [\(\theta \)/f/h]ink aboot it’: An ethnographic investigation of social meaning, social identity and (\(\theta \)) variation in Glasgow. English World-Wide, 35, 68–93. doi: 10.1075/eww.35.1.05law.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Levenson, R. W., Soto, J., & Pole, N. (2007). Emotion, biology, and culture. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 780–796). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  60. Li, J., Wang, L., & Fischer, K. W. (2004). The organization of Chinese shame concepts. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 767–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Lindström, E. (2002). Topics in the grammar of Kuot: A non-Austronesian language of New Ireland, Papua New Guinea. Doctoral dissertation, Univ. StockholmGoogle Scholar
  62. Lutz, C. A. (1982). The domain of emotion words on Ifaluk. American Ethnologist, 9, 113–128. doi: 10.1525/ae.1982.9.1.02a00070.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian Atoll & their challenge to western theory. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  64. Maddieson, I. (2005). Tone. In M. Haspelmath, M. S. Dryer, D. Gil, & B. Comrie (Eds.), World atlas of language structures (pp. 58–61). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  65. Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2008). Words, feelings, and bilingualism: Cross-linguistic differences in emotionality of autobiographical memories. The Mental Lexicon, 3, 72.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  66. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Marshall, M. N. (1996). The key informant technique. Family Practice, 13, 92–97.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Mesquita, B. G. (1993). Cultural variations in emotions: A comparative study of Dutch, Surinamese and Turkish people in the Netherlands. Doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
  69. Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2014). Emotions in context: A sociodynamic model of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 298–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Miller, P. J., & Hoogstra, L. (1992). Language as tool in the socialization and apprehension of cultural meanings. In T. Schwartz, G. M. White, & C. A. Lutz (Eds.), New directions in psychological anthropology (pp. 83–101). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Moen, I. (1993). Functional lateralization of the perception of Norwegian word tones: Evidence from a dichotic listening experiment. Brain and Language, 44, 400–413.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Morling, B., & Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside the head: A meta-analysis of individualism-collectivism in cultural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 199–221. doi: 10.1177/1088868308318260.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Muir, H. (2009). Tonal languages are the key to perfect pitch. New Scientist, 202, 10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Nagy, W., Anderson, R. C., Schommer, M., Scott, J. A., & Stallman, A. C. (1989). Morphological families in the internal lexicon. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 263–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Ogarkova, A., & Soriano, C. (2014). Emotion and the body: A corpus-based investigation of metaphorical containers of anger across languages. International Journal of Cognitive Linguistics, 5, 147–179.Google Scholar
  76. Owusu, G., & Agyei-Mensah, S. (2011). A comparative study of ethnic residential segregation in Ghana’s two largest cities, Accra and Kumasi. Population and Environment, 32, 332–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Pavlenko, A. (2002). Bilingualism and emotions. Multilingua, 21, 45–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Prah, K. K. (2012). The language of development and the development of language in contemporary Africa. Applied Linguistics Review, 3, 295–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1, 60–85. doi: 10.1177/1754073908097189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.426.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Sapir, E. (1921). Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
  83. Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  84. Schieffelin, E. (1983). Anger and shame in the tropical forest: On affect as a cultural system in Papua New Guinea. Ethos, 11, 181–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Shaver, P., & Klinnert, M. (1982). Schachter’s theories of affiliation and emotion: Implications of developmental research. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 37–71.Google Scholar
  86. Silverstein, M. (2006). Old wine, new ethnographic lexicography. Annual Review Of Anthropology, 35, 481–496. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1976). The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: A perspective on the organization of development in infancy. Psychological Review, 83, 173–189.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Taylor, J., & Mbense, T. G. (1998). Red dogs and rotten mealies: How Zulus talk about anger. In Athanasiadou & E. Tabakowska (Eds.), Speaking of emotions: Conceptualization and expression (pp. 191–226). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  89. Tremblay, M. A. (1957). The key informant technique: A nonethnographic application. American Anthropologist, 59, 688–701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Tsai, J. L., Louie, J. Y., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what feelings to desire: Socialization of ideal affect through children’s storybooks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 17–30. doi: 10.1177/0146167206292749.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Tsai, J. L., Simeonova, D. I., & Watanabe, J. T. (2004). Somatic and social: Chinese Americans talk about emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1226–1238.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Tung, M. P. (1994). Symbolic meanings of the body in Chinese culture and “somatization”. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 18, 483–492.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Turpin, M. (2002). Body part terms in Kaytetye feeling expressions. Pragmatics & Cognition, 10, 271–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Semantics, culture, and cognition: Universal human concepts in culture-specific configurations. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  95. Wierzbicka, A. (1994). Emotion, language, and cultural scripts. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 133–196). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Wierzbicka, A. (1996). Semantics: Primes and universals. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  97. Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  98. Wnuk, E., & Majid, A. (2014). Revisiting the limits of language: The odor lexicon of Maniq. Cognition, 131, 125–138.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Yu, C. K. C. (2007). Emotions before, during, and after dreaming sleep. Dreaming, 17, 73–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Zammuner, V. L., & Frijda, N. H. (1994). Felt and communicated emotions: Sadness and jealousy. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 37–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Vivian Dzokoto
    • 1
  • Nicole Senft
    • 2
    Email author
  • Lily Kpobi
    • 3
  • Princess-Melissa Washington-Nortey
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of African American StudiesVirginia Commonwealth UniversityRichmondUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyGeorgetown UniversityWashingtonUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of Ghana School of Medicine & DentistryAccraGhana
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyVirginia Commonwealth UniversityRichmondUSA

Personalised recommendations