Barner, D., Inagaki, S., & Li, P. (2009). Language, thought, and real nouns. Cognition, 111, 329–344.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., & Corina, D. (1991). Linguistic and spatial development: Dissociations between cognitive domains. In N. Krasnegor, D. Rumbaugh, R. Schielfelbusch, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological determinants of language development (pp. 368–398). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2003). Linguistic relativity. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp. 917–921). London: MacMillan Press.
Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2009). How does our language shape the way we think? In M. Brockmman (Ed.), What’s next? Dispatches on the future of science (pp. 116–129). New York: Vintage Press.
Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Schmidt, L. A. (2000). Sex, syntax, and semantics. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 22, 42–47.
Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. A., & Phillips, W. (2003). Sex, syntax, and semantics. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Medow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 61–79). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1996). The origins of children’s spatial semantic categories: Cognitive versus linguistic determinants. In J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 145–176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Brown, R., & Lenneberg, E. (1954). A study in language and cognition. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 454–462.
Article
Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Fias, W., & Marie-Pascale Noe, M.-P. (1998). The Whorfian hypothesis and numerical cognition: Is ‘twenty-four’ processed in the same way as ‘four-and-twenty’? Cognition, 66, 51–77.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Casagrande, J. B. (1958). The function of language classifications in behavior. In E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (pp. 18–31). New York: Henry Holt.
Google Scholar
Chen, J.-Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 427–436.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970–974.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Deutscher, G. (2010). Through the language glass: How words colour your world. London: William Heinemann.
Google Scholar
Elman, J. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179–212.
Article
Google Scholar
Ervin, S. M. (1962). The connotations of gender. Word, 18, 249–261.
Article
Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Furth, H. (1966). Thinking without language. New York: Free Press.
Google Scholar
Gelman, R., & Gallistel, R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Google Scholar
Gentner, G., & Imai, M. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169–200.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Heider, E. R. (1972). Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 10–20.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Hunt, E., & Agnoli, F. (1991). The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychology perspective. Psychological Review, 98, 377–389.
Article
Google Scholar
January, D., & Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 417–426.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Koch, S. C., Zimmermann, F., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2007). El sol-die Sonne: Hat das grammatische Geschlecht von Objekten Implikationen für deren semantischen Gehalt? Psychologische Rundschau, 58, 171–182.
Article
Google Scholar
Konishi, T. (1993). The semantics of grammatical gender: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 519–534.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kousta, S.-T., Vinson, D. P., & Vigliocco, G. (2008). Investigating linguistic relativity through bilingualism: The case of grammatical gender. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 843–858.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.
Article
Google Scholar
Lehmann, T. (1993). A grammar of modern Tamil (2nd ed.). Pondicherry: Pondicherry Institute of Linguistics and Culture.
Google Scholar
Leinbach, M. D., Hort, B. E., & Fagot, B. (1997). Bears are for boys: Metaphorical associations in young children’s gender stereotypes. Cognitive Development, 12, 107–130.
Article
Google Scholar
Levinson, S. L. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: Crosslinguistic evidence. In P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 109–169). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Li, P., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Turing the tables: Language and spatial reasoning. Cognition, 83, 265–294.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lucy, J. (1992). Grammatical categories and cognition: A case study of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York: Liveright.
Google Scholar
Malt, B. C., Sloman, S. A., & Gennari, S. P. (2003). Speaking versus thinking about objects and actions. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Medow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 81–111). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Martinez, I., & Shatz, M. (1996). Linguistic influences on categorization in preschool children: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 23, 529–545.
Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1872/1973). System of logic. In J. M. Robson (Ed.), Collected works of John Stuart Mill (8th ed., Vols. 7 and 8). Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original edition published 1872).
Miller, K., Smith, C., Zhu, J., & Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems. Psychological Science, 6, 56–60.
Article
Google Scholar
Mullen, M. K. (1990). Children’s classifications of nature and artifact pictures into female and male categories. Sex Roles, 23, 577–587.
Article
Google Scholar
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Google Scholar
Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R. T., & du Toit, M. (2011). HLM 7: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International Inc.
Google Scholar
Schiffman, H. F. (1999). A reference grammar of spoken Tamil. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Schiffman, H. F. (2004). The Tamil case system. In J.-L. Chevillard, & E. Wilden (Eds.), South Indian horizons: felicitation volume for Francois Gros on the occasion of his 70th birthday (pp. 293–322). Pondicherry: Institut Francaise d’Indologie de Pondichéry.
Sedlmeier, P., & Renkewitz, F. (2013). Forschungsmethoden und Statistik: Ein Lehrbuch für Psychologen und Sozialwissenschaftler (2nd ed). [Research methods and statistics: A textbook for psychologists and social scientists]. Munich: Pearson Education.
Sera, M. D., Berge, C., & del Castillo Pintado, J. (1994). Grammatical and conceptual forces in the attribution of gender by English and Spanish speakers. Cognitive Development, 9, 261–292.
Article
Google Scholar
Sera, M. D., Elieff, C., Forbes, J., Burch, M. C., & Rodríguez, W. (2002). When language affects cognition and when it does not: An analysis of grammatical gender and classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 377–397.
Article
Google Scholar
Soja, N., Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1991). Ontological categories guide young children’s inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms. Cognition, 45, 101–107.
Article
Google Scholar
Steever, S. B. (1987). Tamil and the Dravidian languages. In B. Comrie (Ed.), The world’s major languages (pp. 725–746). London: Croom Helm.
Google Scholar
Takano, Y. (1989). Methodological problems in cross-cultural studies of linguistic relativity. Cognition, 31, 141–162.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Twain, M. (1880). A tramp abroad. Hartford, CT: American Publishing.
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Paganelli, F., & Dworzynski, K. (2005). Grammatical gender effects on cognition: Implications for language learning and language use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 501–520.
Article
Google Scholar
Vuksanović, J., Bjekić, J., & Radivojević, N. (2014). Grammatical gender and mental representation of object: The case of musical instruments. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. doi:10.1007/s10936-014-9293-7.
Wettler, M., Rapp, R., & Sedlmeier, P. (2005). Free word associations correspond to contiguities between words in texts. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 12, 111–122.
Article
Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1940). Science and linguistics. Technology Review, 42(229–231), 247–248.
Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar