Skip to main content
Log in

Noun Imageability Facilitates the Acquisition of Plurals: Survival Analysis of Plural Emergence in Children

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Some research in child language suggests that semantically general verbs appear in grammatical structures earlier than semantically complex, specific ones. The present study examines whether this was the case in nouns, using imageability as a proxy measure of semantic generality. Longitudinal corpus data from 12 children from the Manchester corpus in CHILDES were used to obtain information on the first occurrence of plurals. A total of 3,560 uninflected nouns were identified in the corpora, of which 1,202 were observed in the plural. Survival analyses indicated that the chance of observing a plural form increases with the imageability rating of the noun, even after accounting for the age of acquisition of the uninflected noun, maternal input frequency, and word length. Noun imageability thus facilitates the acquisition of plural forms. This finding contradicts the observations from verbs, and indicates that the acquisition of grammar is facilitated by high imageability rather than semantic generality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D., & Bates, D. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Lahey, M., Hood, L., Lifter, K., & Fiess, K. (1980). Complex sentences: Acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode. Journal of Child Language, 7, 235–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Lifter, K., & Hafitz, J. (1980). Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language, 56, 386–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., Merkin, S., & Wootten, J. (1982). Wh-questions: Linguistic factors that contribute to the sequence of acquisition. Child Development, 53, 1084–1092.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. (2008). Verb specificity and argument realization in Tzeltal child language. In M. Bowerman & P. Brown (Eds.), Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: Implications for learnability (pp. 167–189). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cameirão, M. L., & Vicente, S. G. (2010). Age-of-acquisition norms for a set of 1,749 Portuguese words. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 474–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1978). Discovering what words can do. In D. Farkas, W. M. Jacobsen, & K. W. Todrys (Eds.), Papers from the parasession on the lexicon (pp. 34–57). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Groot, A. M. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 824–845.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 267–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Bowerman, M. (2009). Why some spatial semantic categories are harder to learn than others: The typological prevalence hypothesis. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, N. Budwig, S. Ervin-Tripp, K. Nakamura, & S. Ozcaliskannm (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 465–480). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilhooly, K., & Logie, R. (1980). Age of acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity and ambiguity measures for 1944 words. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 12, 395–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73, 135–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S., & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 35, 515–531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., & Merves, J. S. (1986). Lexical access for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 92–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, P., & Bowerman, M. (1998). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect in Chinese. First Language, 18, 311–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., McDonough, C., & Tardif, T. (2009). Imageability predicts the age of acquisition of verbs in Chinese children. Journal of Child Language, 36, 405–423.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2008). Enriching CHILDES for morphosyntactic analysis. In H. Behrens (Ed.), Corpora in language acquisition research: history, methods, perspectives. New York: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maouene, J., Laakso, A., & Smith, L. B. (2011). Object associations of early-learned light and heavy English verbs. First Language, 31, 109–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., & Xu, F. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monogrraphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, 1–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masterson, J., Druks, J., & Gallienne, D. (2008). Object and action picture naming in three- and five-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 35, 373–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFalls, E. L., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Stahl, S. A. (1996). Influence of word meaning on the acquisition of a reading vocabulary in second-grade children. Reading and Writing, 8, 235–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Section A): Human Experimental Psychology, 50, 528–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1999). Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. Journal Of Child Language, 26, 619–653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968) Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology 76(1, Pt. 2):1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parisi, D., & Antinucci, F. (1970). Lexical competence. In W. J. M. Levelt & G B Fd d’ Arcais (Eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics (pp. 197–210). Amsterdam: North Holland.

  • Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prado, E. L., & Ullman, M. T. (2009). Can imageability help us draw the line between storage and composition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 35, 849–866.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar, M. (2002). The role of meaning in inflection: Why the past tense does not require a rule. Cognitive Psychology, 45(1), 45–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J., & Kean, J. (2007). Formal distinctiveness of high- and low-imageability nouns: Analyses and theoretical implications. Cognitive Science, 31, 157–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., & Bode, J. V. (1993). GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. First Language, 13, 113–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V. M., & Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants of acquisition order in wh-questions: Re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech. Journal of Child Language, 30, 609–635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serratrice, L., Joseph, K. L., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). The acquisition of past tense in preschool children with specific language impairment and unaffected controls: Regular and irregular forms. Linguistics, 41, 321–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shirai, Y. (2010). Semantic bias and morphological regularity in the acquisition of tense-aspect morphology: what is the relation? Linguistics, 48, 171–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 175–208). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephany, U. (1981). Verbal grammar in modern Greek early child language. In P. S. Dale & D. Ingram (Eds.), Child language: An international perspective (pp. 45–57). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1140–1154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Kahana-Kalman, R., Baumwell, L., & Cyphers, L. (1998). Predicting variation in the timing of language milestones in the second year: An events history approach. Journal of Child Language, 25, 675–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2001). The role of performance limitations in the acquisition of verb-argument structure: An alternative account. Journal of Child Language, 28, 127–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language, 31, 61–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Therneau, T. M., & Grambsch, P. M. (2000). Modeling survival data: Extending the Cox model. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Therneau, T. M., & Lumley, T. (2009) Survival: Survival analysis, including penalised likelihood. [Computer software manual]. (Available from: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=survival)

  • Toglia, M., & Battig, W. (1978). Handbook of semantic word norms. New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine readable dictionary, version 2. Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 20, 6–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Filip Smolík.

Additional information

The study was supported by the Czech Science Foundation (GA ČR) award No. P407/10/2047 Comprehension of grammar and lexicon in toddlers

Appendix

Appendix

A. List of top 21 words in imageability in the analyzed sample (imageability \(>\) 632): apple, banana, beach, bed, beetle, car, dog, father, fire, girl, gorilla, ice, kitten, milk, pickle, pig, puppy, skin, squirrel, sun, telephone.

B. List of words with imageability \(<\) 500 (excluded in the first reanalysis from Table 4): age, answer, back, balance, bang, belt, bit, block, blow, bottom, brave, break, build, bump, buy, care, clean, cool, count, course, cover, crawl, creature, crumb, crush, cry, cut, cymbal, damage, die, dip, down, dream, drop, end, escape, fault, feel, find, finish, front, germ, give, guess, help, hide, hold, hurt, idea, inch, job, keep, last, lead, lie, life, lift, line, load, look, lorry, manner, march, mark, match, matter, mean, measure, mind, minute, miss, move, name, need, number, open, pass, pat, pattern, peck, peel, peep, piece, place, play, present, pull, push, race, reach, repair, rescue, rest, ride, roll, rush, shape, shoot, shout, show, side, slide, slip, smell, snap, sound, squeak, squirt, stand, stay, step, still, stop, story, stroke, surprise, sweet, take, talk, taste, thing, throw, till, time, tip, top, touch, tow, track, trick, trouble, try, tumble, turn, vacuum, wait, way, wear, week, whack, win, wish, work, wrong, year.

C. List of the semantically or categorically suspicious words excluded in second reanalysis from Table 4: blue, boat, care, cook, down, drink, end, fall, find, fire, fly, give, green, jump, keep, kiss, light, look, love, move, need, open, paint, post, rain, slide, stay, step, stick, stop, sweet, take, top, turn, walk, wear, whistle, white, work, yellow, drill, hurt, idea, pan, pea, pin, bubble, squeak, pump, try, puzzle, cover, matter, clock.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Smolík, F. Noun Imageability Facilitates the Acquisition of Plurals: Survival Analysis of Plural Emergence in Children. J Psycholinguist Res 43, 335–350 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9255-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9255-5

Keywords

Navigation