Skip to main content
Log in

The Changing Role of Gesture in Linguistic Development: A Developmental Trajectory and a Cross-Cultural Comparison Between British and Finnish Children

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We studied how gesture use changes with culture, age and increased spoken language competence. A picture-naming task was presented to British (N = 80) and Finnish (N = 41) typically developing children aged 2–5 years. British children were found to gesture more than Finnish children and, in both cultures, gesture production decreased after the age of two. Two-year-olds used more deictic than iconic gestures than older children, and gestured more before the onset of speech, rather than simultaneously or after speech. The British 3- and 5-year-olds gestured significantly more when naming praxic (manipulable) items than non-praxic items. Our results support the view that gesture serves a communicative and intrapersonal function, and the relative function may change with age. Speech and language therapists and psychologists observe the development of children’s gestures and make predictions on the basis of their frequency and type. To prevent drawing erroneous conclusions about children’s linguistic development, it is important to understand developmental and cultural variations in gesture use.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allwood, J. (1999). Are there Swedish patterns of communication? In H. Tamura (Ed.), Cultural acceptance of CSCW in Japan & Nordic countries (pp. 90–120). Kyoto: Kyoto Institute of Technology. Retrieved from http://www.ling.gu.se/~jens/publications/docs076-100/087.pdf.

  • Bates E., Dick F. (2002) Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology 40(3): 293–310

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bello A., Capirci O., Volterra V. (2004) Lexical production in children with Williams syndrome: Spontaneous use of gesture in a naming task. Neuropsychologia 42: 201–213

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake J., Myszczyszyn D., Jokel A., Bebroglu N. (2008) Gestures accompanying speech in specifically language-impaired children and their timing with speech. First Language 28(2): 237–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capirci O., Volterra V. (2008) Gesture and speech—The emergence and development of a strong and changing partnership. Gesture 8(1): 22–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capone N. C. (2007) Tapping toddlers’ evolving semantic representation via gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50(3): 732–745

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carbaugh D. A. (2005) Cultures in conversation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbaugh D. A., Berry M., Nurmikari-Berry M. (2006) Coding personhood through cultural terms and practices. Silence and quietude as a Finnish “natural way of being”. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 25(3): 1–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis M. (2002) From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson M., Berglund E. (1999) Swedish early communicative development inventories: Words and gestures. First Language 19(55): 55–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson L., Dale P., Reznick J., Bates E., Thal D., & Pethick S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development. Serial No. 242, 59.

  • Gentilucci M., Dalla Volta R., Gianelli C. (2008) When the hands speak. Journal of Physiology-Paris 102(1–3): 21–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • German D. (1984) Diagnosis of word-finding disorders in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities 17(6): 353–359

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S. (2003) Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Belknap Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S., Saltzman J. (2000) The cultural bounds of maternal accommodation: How Chinese and American mothers communicate with deaf and hearing children. Psychologial Science 11(4): 307–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson J. M., Braddock B. A. (2011) Gesture and motor skill in relation to language in children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 54(1): 72–86

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson J. M., Capirci O., Longobardi E., Caselli M. C. (1999) Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development 14(1): 57–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson J. M., Capirci O., Volterra V., Goldin-Meadow S. (2008) Learning to talk in gesture-rich world: Early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language 28(2): 164–181

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson J. M., Goldin-Meadow S. (1997) What’s communication got to do with it? Gesture in children blind from birth. Developmental Psychology 33(3): 453–467

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson J. M., Goldin-Meadow S. (2001) The resilience of gesture in talk: Gestures in blind speakers and listeners. Developmental Science 4(4): 416–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakkula, K. (2002). Esineiden antaminen. Kehityksen peili ja kieltä ennakoiva sosiaalinen merkki 9-34 kuukauden iässä (Giving objects. A mirror of development and a social sign anticipating language acquisition of 9-34 months old children). Acta Universitatis Ouluensis, Series E, Scientiae Rerum Socialium 52. University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland. Retrieved from http://herkules.oulu.fi/isbn9514266536/isbn9514266536.pdf.

  • Karmiloff-Smith A. (1996) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kita S. (2009) Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2): 145–167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laakso M., Helasvuo M.-L., Savinainen-Makkonen T. (2010a) Children’s early actions in learning language: A study of proto-words and pointing gestures in interaction between one-year-old child and parent. SKY Journal of Linguistics 23: 199–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Laakso, M.-L., Eklund, K., & Poikkeus, A.-M. (2010b). Lapsen esikielellisen kommunikaation ja kielen ensikartoitus (Survey of the child’s preverbal communication and language). Jyväskylä: Niilo Mäki Instituutti.

  • Laakso M.-L., Poikkeus A.-M., Katajamäki J., Lyytinen P. (1999) Early intentional communication as a predictor of language development in young toddlers. First Language 19(56): 207–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt W. J. M. (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt W. J. M., Roelofs A., Meyer A. S. (1989) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(1): 1–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Lock A., Young A., Service V., Chandler P. (1994) Some observations on the origins of the pointing gesture. In: Volterra V., Erting C. J. (eds) From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Gallaudet University Press, Washington, DC, pp 42–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry R. I., Nicoladis E. (2000) Gesture reflects language development: Evidence from bilingual children. Current Directions in Psychological Science 9(6): 192–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCroskey J. C., Fayer J. M., Richmond V. P., Sallinen A., Barraclough R. A. (1996) A multi-cultural examination of the relationship between nonverbal immediacy and affective learning. Communication Quarterly 44(3): 297–307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mäkisalo A. (1988) Suomalainen kommunikaatio ulkomaalaisen silmin (Finnish communication as seen by a foreigner). Opettaja 83(10): 20–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicoladis E. (2002) Some gestures develop in conjunction with spoken language development and others don’t: Evidence from bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 26(4): 241–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicoladis E., Pika S., Marentette P. (2009)    Do French–English bilingual children gesture more than monolingual children?. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 38(6): 573–585

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow S., Goldin-Meadow S. (2009) When gesture speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2): 190–217

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozyürek A., Kita S., Allen S., Brown A., Furman R., Ishizuka T. (2008) Development of cross-linguistic variation in speech and gesture: Motion events in English and Turkish. Developmental Psychology 44(4): 1040–1054

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pika S., Nicoladis E., Marentette P. (2006) A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9(3): 319–327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pine K. J., Bird H., Kirk E. (2007a) The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science 10(6): 747–754

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pine K. J., Gurney D. J., Fletcher B. C. (2010) The semantic specificity hypothesis: When gestures do not depend upon the presence of a listener. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour 34(3): 169–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pine K. J., Lufkin N., Kirk E., Messer D. (2007b) A microgenetic analysis of the relationship between speech and gesture in children: Evidence for semantic and temporal asynchrony. Language and Cognitive Processes 22(2): 234–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pine K. J., Lufkin N., Messer D. J. (2004) More gestures than answers: Children learning about balance. Developmental Psychology 40(6): 1059–1067

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poutiainen, S. (2007). Finnish cultural discourses about mobile phone communication. Doctoral dissertation. University of Massachusetts Amherst, Department of Communication. Retrieved from http://www.helsinki.fi/puhetieteet/henkilokunta/poutiainen/dissertation.pdf.

  • Rauscher F. H., Krauss R. M., Chen Y. (1996) Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science 7(4): 226–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remes K. (1975) Artikulaatiotesti. Kuvakortisto puheenkehityksen ja äännevirheisyyden tutkimiseen ja tarkkailuun. Koulun Erityispalvelu, Helsinki

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe M. L., Özçalişkan S., Goldin-Meadow S. (2008) Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language 28(2): 182–199

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sajavaara, K., & Lehtonen, J. (1997). The silent Finn revisited. In A. Jaworski (Ed.), Silence. Interdisclipinary perspectives (pp. 263–283). Studies in Anthropological Linguistics, 10. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

  • Sallinen-Kuparinen, A. (1986). Finnish communication reticence: Perceptions and self-reported behavior. Studia philologica Jyväskyläensia, 19. Jyväskylä (Finland): University of Jyväskylä.

  • Stefanini S., Caselli M. C., Volterra V. (2007) Spoken and gestural production in a naming task by young children with Down syndrome. Brain and Language 101(3): 208–221

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanini S., Bello A., Caselli M. C., Iverson J., Volterra V. (2009) Co-speech gestures in a naming task: Developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes 24(2): 168–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thal D., Tobias S., Morrison D. (1991) Language and gestures in late talkers. A 1-year follow up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 34(3): 604–612

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thurnham A. J., Pine K. J. (2006) The effects of single and dual representations on children’s gesture production. Cognitive Development 21(1): 46–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulviste T., Mizera L., De Geer B., Tryggvason M.-T. (2003) A silent Finn, a silent Finno–Ugric, or a silent Nordic? A comparative study of Estonian, Finnish, and Swedish mother-adolescent interactions. Applied Psycholinguistics 24(2): 249–265

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulviste, T. (2004). Mothers’ conversational styles across cultures. The cases of Estonia, Finland, Sweden, and the U.S. In B. N. Setiadi, A. Supratiknya, W. J. Lonner, & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), Ongoing themes in psychology and culture (Online Ed.). Melbourne, FL: International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology. Retrieved from http://ebooks.iaccp.org/ongoing_themes/.

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (2002). Communication and symbolic behavior scales developmental profile. First normed edition. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

  • Wiik K. (1977) Suomen tavuista (On Finnish syllables). Virittäjä 81(3): 265–277

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to K. H. Huttunen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Huttunen, K.H., Pine, K.J., Thurnham, A.J. et al. The Changing Role of Gesture in Linguistic Development: A Developmental Trajectory and a Cross-Cultural Comparison Between British and Finnish Children. J Psycholinguist Res 42, 81–101 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-012-9205-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-012-9205-7

Keywords

Navigation