Skip to main content
Log in

On the Nature of Semantic Constraints on Lexical Access

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We present two eye-tracking experiments that investigate lexical frequency and semantic context constraints in spoken-word recognition in German. In both experiments, the pivotal words were pairs of nouns overlapping at onset but varying in lexical frequency. In Experiment 1, German listeners showed an expected frequency bias towards high-frequency competitors (e.g., Blume, ‘flower’) when instructed to click on low-frequency targets (e.g., Bluse, ‘blouse’). In Experiment 2, semantically constraining context increased the availability of appropriate low-frequency target words prior to word onset, but did not influence the availability of semantically inappropriate high-frequency competitors at the same time. Immediately after target word onset, however, the activation of high-frequency competitors was reduced in semantically constraining sentences, but still exceeded that of unrelated distractor words significantly. The results suggest that (1) semantic context acts to downgrade activation of inappropriate competitors rather than to exclude them from competition, and (2) semantic context influences spoken-word recognition, over and above anticipation of upcoming referents.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allopenna P., Magnuson J., Tanenhaus M. (1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language 38: 419–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann G., Kamide Y. (1999) Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition 73: 247–264

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Altmann G., Kamide Y. (2007) The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language 57: 502–518

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.

  • Barr D. J. (2008) Pragmatic expectations and linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground. Cognition 109: 18–40

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blutner R., Sommer R. (1988) Sentence processing and lexical access: The influence of the focus-identifying task. Journal of Memory and Language 27: 359–367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonin P., Barry C., Meot A., Chalard M. (2004) The influence of age of acquisition in word reading and other tasks: A never ending story?. Journal of Memory and Language 50: 456–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connine C., Blasko D., Wang J. (1994) Vertical similarity in spoken word recognition: The role of sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language 32: 193–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connine C., Mullennix J., Shernoff E., Yelen J. (1990) Word familiarity and frequency in visual and auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 1084–1096

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connine C., Titone D., Wang J. (1993) Auditory word recognition: Extrinsic and intrinsic effects of word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19: 81–94

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper R. (1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language. Cognitive Psychology 6: 84–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan D., Gaskell M. (2007) The temporal dynamics of ambiguity resolution: Evidence from spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 57: 483–501

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan D., Magnuson J., Tanenhaus M. (2001) Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology 42: 317–367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan D., Swingley D., Tanenhaus M. K., Magnuson J. S. (2000) Linguistic gender and spoken-word recognition in French. Journal of Memory and Language 42: 465–480

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahan D., Tanenhaus M. (2004) Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken language comprehension: Immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30: 498–513

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy S. A., Morris R. K., Rayner K. (1988) Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading. Journal of Memory and Language 27: 429–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux E., Mehler J. (1990) Monitoring the lexicon with normal and compressed speech: Frequency effects and the prelexical code. Journal of Memory and Language 29: 316–335

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eberhard K., Spivey-Knowlton S., Sedivy J., Tanenhaus M. (1995) Eye movements as a window into real-time spoken language processing in natural contexts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24: 409–436

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiebach C. J., Friederici A. D., Müller K., von Cramon D. Y., Hernandez A. E. (2003) Distinct brain responses for early and late learned words. Neuroimage 19: 1627–1637

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell M., Marslen-Wilson W. (1997) Integrating form and meaning: A distributed model of speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes 12: 613–656

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell M., Marslen-Wilson W. (2002) Representation and competition in the perception of spoken words. Cognitive Psychology 45: 220–266

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallet P. (1986) Eye movements. In: Boff K., Kaufman L., Thomas J. (eds) Handbook of perception and human performance. Wiley, New York, pp 10-1–10-112

    Google Scholar 

  • Huettig F., Altmann G. (2004) The online processing of ambiguous and unambiguous words in context: Evidence from head-mounted eye-tracking. In: Carreiras M., Clifton C. (eds) The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP and beyond. Psychology Press, New York, NY, pp 187–207

    Google Scholar 

  • Huettig F., Altmann G. (2007) Visual-shape competition during language-mediated attention is based on lexical input and not modulated by contextual appropriateness. Visual Cognition 15: 985–1018

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huettig F., McQueen J. (2007) The tug of war between phonological, semantic, and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language 57: 460–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kambe G., Rayner K., Duffy S. A. (2001) Global context effects on processing lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye fixations. Memory & Cognition 29: 363–372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamide Y., Altmann G., Haywood S. (2003) The time course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 49: 133–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas M. (1987) Frequency effects on the processing of ambiguous words in sentence contexts. Language and Speech 30: 25–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Luce P., Goldinger S., Auer E., Vitevitch M. (2000) Phonetic priming, neighborhood activation, and PARSYN. Perception & Psychophysics 62: 615–625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luce P., Pisoni D. (1998) Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing 19: 1–36

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson W. (1989) Access and integration: Projecting sounds onto meaning. In: Marslen-Wilson W. (eds) Lexical representation and process. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 3–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson W. (1990) Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In: Altmann G. T. M. (eds) Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 148–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro D. (1996) Integration of multiple sources of information in language processing. In: Inui T., McClelland J. (eds) Attention and performance XVI. Information integration in perception and communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 397–432

    Google Scholar 

  • Matin E., Shao K., Boff K. (1993) Saccadic overhead: Information processing time with and without saccades. Perceptual Psychophysics 53: 372–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland J., Elman J. (1986) The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology 10: 1–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McQueen J. M. (1993) Rhyme decisions to spoken words and nonwords. Memory & Cognition 21: 210–222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller J., Eimas P. (1995) Speech perception: From signal to word. Annual Review of Psychology 46: 467–492

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moss H., Marslen-Wilson W. (1993) Access to word meanings during spoken language comprehension: Effects of sentential semantic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition 19: 1254–1276

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris D., Cutler A., McQueen J., Butterfield S. (2006) Phonological and conceptual activation in speech comprehension. Cognitive Psychology 523: 146–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris D., McQueen J. (2008) Shortlist B: A Bayesian model of continuous speech recognition. Psychological Review 115: 357–395

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oden G., Spira J. (1983) Influence of context on the activation and selection of ambiguous word senses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 35: 51–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Onifer W., Swinney D. (1981) Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: Effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias. Memory & Cognition 9: 225–236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulte im Walde S. (2008) Human associations and the choice of features for semantic verb classification. Research on Language and Computation 6: 79–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg M., Tanenhaus M., Leiman J., Bienkowski M. (1982) Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology 14: 489–537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sereno S. C., Pacht J. M., Rayner K. (1992) Effect of meaning frequency on processing lexically ambiguous words. Psychological Science 3: 296–300

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson G. (1981) Meaning dominance and semantic context in the processing of lexical ambiguity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20: 120–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swinney D. (1979) Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18: 645–659

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi P. (1988) Effects of context on the immediate interpretation of unambiguous nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 14: 153–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi P., Colombo L., Job R. (1987) Accessing lexical ambiguity: Effects of context and dominance. Psychological Research 49: 161–167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabossi P., Zardon F. (1993) Processing ambiguous words in context. Journal of Memory and Language 32: 359–372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus M., Donenwerth-Nolan S. (1984) Syntactic context and lexical access. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 36: 649–661

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus M., Leiman J., Seidenberg M. (1979) Evidence for multiple stages in the processing of ambiguous words in syntactic contexts. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18: 427–440

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch M. (2002) Influence of onset density on spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28: 270–278

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch M., Luce P. (1998) When words compete: Levels of processing in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science 9: 325–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitevitch M., Luce P. (1999) Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 40: 374–408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber A., Cutler A. (2004) Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 50: 1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, A., Melinger, A. (2008). Name dominance in spoken word recognition is (not) modulated by expectations: evidence from synonyms. In Proceedings of the international speech communication association workshop: Experimental linguistics (pp. 205–208). Athens, Greece.

  • Whitney P., McKay T., Kellas G., Emerson W. (1985) Semantic activation of noun concepts in context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11: 126–135

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood P. (1989) The locus of the effects of sentential-semantic context in spoken-word processing. Cognition 32: 25–64

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood P., Schriefers H. (1995) Effects of sensory information and processing time in spoken-word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes 10: 121–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrea Weber.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Weber, A., Crocker, M.W. On the Nature of Semantic Constraints on Lexical Access. J Psycholinguist Res 41, 195–214 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9184-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9184-0

Keywords

Navigation