Skip to main content
Log in

How Readers Spontaneously Interpret Man-Suffix Words: Evidence from Eye Movements

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated whether readers are more likely to assign a male referent to man-suffix terms (e.g. chairman) than to gender-neutral alternatives (e.g., chairperson) during reading, and whether this bias differs as a function of age. Younger and older adults’ eye movements were monitored while reading passages containing phrases such as “The chairman/chairperson familiarized herself with…” On-line eye fixation data provided strong evidence that man-suffix words were more likely to evoke the expectation of a male referent in both age groups. Younger readers demonstrated inflated processing times when first encountering herself after chairman relative to chairperson, and they tended to make more regressive fixations to chairman. Older readers did not show the effect when initially encountering herself, but they spent disproportionately longer looking back to chairman and herself. The study provides empirical support for copy-editing policies that mandate the use of explicitly gender-neutral suffix terms in place of man-suffix terms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • APA Publication Manual Task Force: (1977) Guidelines for nonsexist language in APA journals: Publication Manual change sheet 2. American Psychologist 3(2): 487–494

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes P. B. (1997) On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny. American Psychologist 52: 366–380

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman M., Hannon B., Burton C. (2006) Are there age-related differences in shallow semantic processing of text? Evidence from eye movements. Discourse Processes 42: 177–203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman M., Lennertz T., Hannon B. (2007) Shallow semantic processing of text: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes 22: 83–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy S. A., Keir J. A. (2004) Violating stereotypes: Eye movements and comprehension processes when text conflicts with world knowledge. Memory & Cognition 32: 551–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy S. A., Rayner K. (1990) Eye movements and anaphor resolution: Effect of antecedent typicality and distance. Language & Speech 33: 1003–1019

    Google Scholar 

  • Harper & Row Publishers Inc: (1976) Harper & Row guidelines on equal treatment of the sexes in textbooks. Harper & Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Holt, Rinehart & Winston (College Division): (1976) The treatment of sex roles and minorities. Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson R. E. (2003) Aging and the remembering of text. Developmental Review 23: 261–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanfer S. (1972) Sispeak: A Ms-guided attempt to change herstory. Time 100(October 23): 79

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy D. (1993) Nonsexist language: A progress report. Canadian Journal of Education 18: 223–238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreiner H., Sturt P., Garrod S. (2008) Processing definitional and stereotypical gender in reference resolution: Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language 58: 239–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay D. G. (1980) Psychology, prescriptive grammar, and the pronoun problem. American Psychologist 35: 444–449

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martyna W. (1980) Beyond the “he/man” approach: The case for nonsexist language. Signs 5: 482–493

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConnell A. R., Fazio R. H. (1996) Women as men and people: Effects of gender-marked language. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22: 1004–1013

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt R. D., Kok C. J. (1995) Attribution of gender to a gender-unspecified individual: An evaluation of the people = male hypothesis. Sex Roles 33: 145–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulton J., Robinson G. M., Elias C. (1978) Sex bias in language use: “Neutral” pronouns that aren’t. American Psychologist 33: 1032–1036

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy D., Daneman M., Schneider B. A. (2006) Why do older adults have difficulty following conversations?. Psychology and Aging 21: 49–61

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout L., Bersick M., McLaughlin J. (1997) Brain potentials reflect violations of gender stereotypes. Memory & Cognition 25: 273–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice-Hall Inc: (1975) Prentice-Hall author’s guide (5th ed.). Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Random House: (1975) Guidelines for multiethnic/nonsexist survey. Random House, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven J. C. (1965) The Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale. Lewis, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner K., Duffy S. A. (1986) Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition 14: 191–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin D. L., Greene K., Schneider J. W. (1994) Adopting gender-inclusive language reforms. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 13: 91–114

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider B. A., Daneman M., Murphy D. (2005) Speech comprehension difficulties in older adults: Cognitive slowing or age-related changes in hearing?. Psychology and Aging 20: 261–271

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider J. W., Hacker S. L. (1973) Sex role imagery and the use of generic “man” in introductory texts: A case in the sociology of sociology. American Sociologist 8: 12–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Shen J., Reingold E. M., Pomplun M. (2003) Guidance of eye movements during conjunctive visual search: The distractor-ratio effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 57: 76–96

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sturt P. (2003) The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 542–562

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Horne, H. (1976). Women’s movement foolishly assaults the English language. Rocky Mountain News (February 19), 51.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meredyth Daneman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Khan, M., Daneman, M. How Readers Spontaneously Interpret Man-Suffix Words: Evidence from Eye Movements. J Psycholinguist Res 40, 351 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9173-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9173-3

Keywords

Navigation