Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of the Phonological Loop in English Word Learning: A Comparison of Chinese ESL Learners and Native Speakers

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although the role of the phonological loop in word-retention is well documented, research in Chinese character retention suggests the involvement of non-phonological encoding. This study investigated whether the extent to which the phonological loop contributes to learning and remembering visually introduced words varies between college-level Chinese ESL learners (N = 20) and native speakers of English (N = 20). The groups performed a paired associative learning task under two conditions (control versus articulatory suppression) with two word types (regularly spelled versus irregularly spelled words) differing in degree of phonological accessibility. The results demonstrated that both groups’ recall declined when the phonological loop was made less available (with irregularly spelled words and in the articulatory suppression condition), but the decline was greater for the native group. These results suggest that word learning entails phonological encoding uniformly across learners, but the contribution of phonology varies among learners with diverse linguistic backgrounds.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akamatsu N. (2002) A similarity in word-recognition procedures among second language readers with different first language backgrounds. Applied Psycholinguistics 23: 117–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akamatsu N. (2003) The effects of first language orthographic features on second language reading in text. Language Learning 53: 207–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A. D. (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford, England

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley A., Gathercole S., Papagno C. (1998) The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review 105: 158–173

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer D. W., Stanovich K. E. (1980) Lexical access and the spelling-to-sound regularity effect. Memory & Cognition 8: 424–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey J. A., Francis J. (1991) Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics 12: 91–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown T., Haynes M. (1985) Literacy background and reading development in a second language. In: Carr T.H. (eds) The development of reading skills. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp 19–34

    Google Scholar 

  • DeFrancis J. (1989) Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing system. University of Hawai’i, Honolulu

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis N. C., Beaton A. (1993) Psycholinguistic determinants of foreign language vocabulary learning. Language Learning 43: 559–617

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fender M. (2003) English word recognition and word integration skills of native Arabic- and Japanese-speaking learners of English as a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 289–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gass, S. M., Selinker, L. (eds) (1983) Language transfer in language learning. Newbury House, Rowley, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole S.E., Hitch G. J., Adams A.-M., Martin A. J. (1999) Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology 13: 65–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glushko R. J. (1979) The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13: 104–115

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamada M., Koda K. (2008) Influence of first language orthographic experience on second language decoding and word learning. Language Learning 58: 1–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington M., Sawyer M. (1992) L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 14: 25–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes M., Carr T. H. (1990) Writing system background and second language reading: A component skills analysis of English reading by native speaker-readers of Chinese. In: Carr T. H., Levy B. A. (eds) Reading and its development: Component skills approaches. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 375–421

    Google Scholar 

  • Hue C.-W., Erickson J. R. (1988) Short-term memory for Chinese characters and radicals. Memory & Cognition 16: 196–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared D. (1997) Spelling-sound consistency affects the naming of high-frequency words. Journal of Memory and Language 36: 505–529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared D. (2002) Spelling-sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language 46: 723–750

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kay J., Marcel A. (1981) One process, not two, in reading aloud: Lexical analogies do the work of non-lexical rules. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 33: 397–413

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellerman, E., Sharwood Smith, M. (eds) (1986) Crosslinguistic influence in second language acquisition. Pergamon Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda K. (1995) Cognitive consequences of L1 and L2 orthographies. In: Tayor I., Olson D. R. (eds) Scripts and literacy. Kluwer Academic Publishers, The Netherlands, pp 311–326

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovács G., Racsmány M. (2008) Handling L1 input in phonological STM: The effect of non-L1 phonetic segments and non-L1 phonotactics on nonword repetition. Language Learning 58: 597–624

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kucera H., Francis W.H. (1967) Computational analysis of present-day American English. Brown University Press, Providence, RI

    Google Scholar 

  • Laufer B. (1997) What’s in a word that makes it hard or easy: Some intralexical factors that affect the learning of words. In: Schmitt N., McCarthy M. (eds) Vocabulary: Description, acquisition, and pedagogy. Cambridge University Press, England, pp 140–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattingly I. G. (1992) Linguistic awareness and orthographic form. In: Frost R., Katz L. (eds) Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, pp 11–26

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang C., Cho J., Liu H., Wagner R. K., Shu H., Zhou A., Cheuk C. S.-M., Muse A. (2005) Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 92: 140–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mori Y. (1998) Effects of first language and phonological accessibility on kanji recognition. The Modern Language Journal 82: 69–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mou L.-C., Anderson N. S. (1981) Graphemic and phonemic codings of Chinese characters in short-term retention. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17: 255–258

    Google Scholar 

  • Muljani D., Koda K., Moates D. R. (1998) The development of word recognition in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics 19: 99–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation I. P. S. (2001) Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge University Press, England

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris D., Brown G. (1985) Race models and analogy theories: A dead heat? Reply to Seidenberg. Cognition 20: 155–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papagno C., Valentine T., Baddeley A. (1991) Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 331–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papagno C., Vallar G. (1992) Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words: The effect of phonological similarity and item length. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 44: 47–67

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkin A. J., Underwood G. (1983) Orthographic vs. phonological irregularity in lexical decision. Memory and Cognition 11: 351–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A., Beck I., Bell L. C., Hughes C. (1987) Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33: 283–319

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A., Tan L. H. (1998) The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24: 101–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A., Zhang S. (1991) Phonological processes in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17: 633–643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A., Zhang S. (1995) Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 24–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C.A., Zhang S., Berent I. (1992) Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a “universal” phonological principle. In: Frost R., Katz L. (eds) Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 227–248

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saito S., Logie R., Morita A., Law A. (2008) Visual and phonological similarity effects in verbal immediate serial recall: A test with kanji characters. Journal of Memory and Language 59: 1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg M. S., Waters G. S., Barnes M. A., Tanenhaus M. K. (1984) When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition?. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23: 383–404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Service E. (1992) Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 45: 21–50

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Service E., Kohonen V. (1995) Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition?. Applied Psycholinguistics 16: 155–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solso R.L., Juel C.L. (1980) Positional frequency and versatility of bigrams for two- through nine-letter English words. Instrumentation & Techniques 12: 297–343

    Google Scholar 

  • Speciale G., Ellis N. C., Bywater T. (2004) Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 25: 293–321

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinks J. A., Liu Y., Perfetti C. A., Tan L. H. (2000) Reading Chinese characters for meaning: The role of phonological information. Cognition 76: B1–B11

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tan L. H., Hoosain R., Siok W. W. T. (1996) Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22: 865–882

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan L. H., Perfetti C. A. (1997) Visual Chinese character recognition: Does phonological information mediate access to meaning?. Journal of Memory and Language 37: 41–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taraban R., McClelland J. L. (1987) Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Language 26: 608–631

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavassoli N. T. (2002) Spatial memory for Chinese and English. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology 33: 415–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky L. (2003) The cradle of culture and what children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F. R., Scanlon D. M. (1987) Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33: 321–363

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang M., Koda K. (2005) Commonalities and differences in word identification skills among learners of English as a second language. Language Learning 55: 71–98

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang M., Koda K., Perfetti C. A. (2003) Alphabetic and nonalphabetic L1 effects in English word identification: A comparison of Korean and Chinese English L2 learners. Cognition 87: 129–149

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yik W. F. (1978) The effect of visual and acoustic similarity on short-term memory for Chinese words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 30: 487–494

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler J. C., Goswami U. (2005) Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin 131: 3–29

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler J. C., Goswami U. (2006) Becoming literate in different languages: Similar problems, different solutions. Developmental Science 9: 429–453

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang G., Simon H. (1985) STM capacity for Chinese words and idioms: Chunking and acoustical loop hypotheses. Memory & Cognition 13: 193–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Megumi Hamada.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hamada, M., Koda, K. The Role of the Phonological Loop in English Word Learning: A Comparison of Chinese ESL Learners and Native Speakers. J Psycholinguist Res 40, 75–92 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-010-9156-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-010-9156-9

Keywords

Navigation