Skip to main content
Log in

A Longitudinal Characterization of Typical Laughter Development in Mother–Child Interaction from 12 to 36 Months: Formal Features and Reciprocal Responsiveness

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Laughter is a valuable means for communicating and engaging in interaction since the earliest months of life. Nevertheless, there is a dearth of work on how its use develops in early interactions—given its putative reflexive nature, it has often been disregarded from studies on pre-linguistic vocalizations. We provide a longitudinal characterization of laughter use analyzing interactions of 4 babies with their mothers at five time-points (12, 18, 24, 30, and 36 months). We show how child laughter is very distinct from mothers’ (and adults’ generally), in terms of frequency, duration, level of arousal displayed, overlap with speech, and responsiveness to others’ laughter. Notably, contrary to what might be expected, we observed that children laugh significantly less than their mothers, especially at the first time-points analyzed. We indeed observe an increasing developmental trajectory in the production of laughter overall and in the contingent multimodal response to mothers’ laughter, showing the child’s increasing attunement to the social environment, interest in others’ appraisals and mental states, and awareness of its communicative value. We also show how mothers’ contingent responses to child laughter change over time, going from high-frequency mimicry, to a lower rate of diversified multimodal responses, in line with the child’s neuro-psychological development. Our data support a dynamic view of dialogue where interactants influence each other bidirectionally and emphasizes the crucial communicative value of laughter. When language is not fully developed, laughter might be an early means, in its already fully available expressiveness, to hold the conversational turn and enable meaningful vocal contribution in interaction at the same level of the interlocutor. Our study aims to provide a benchmark for typical laughter development, since we believe it can be an early means, along with other commonly analyzed behaviors (e.g., smiling, gazing, pointing, etc.), to gain insight into early child neuro-psychological development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of Data and Materials

The videos annotated for the current work are all publicly available on the CHILDES database at https://phonbank.talkbank.org/access/Eng-NA/Providence.html. Our full annotation dataset, all indexed videoclips referred to throughout the manuscript, and materials related to the statistical analysis are available at: https://osf.io/48fmd/?view_only=a3bcf69ab71f43fdac820b1e46c6e5de.

Code Availability

The R scripts used for the statistical analysis are available at https://osf.io/48fmd/?view_only=a3bcf69ab71f43fdac820b1e46c6e5de.

Notes

  1. In the current paper, by the term mimicry we mean the production of a behavior shortly after the partner that is type identical in certain dimensions, as used in Mayo and Gordon (2020) and El Haddad et al. (2019), and reviewed in Chartrand and Lakin (2013).

  2. We excluded Ethan from our study because he was later diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome and no videos were available for annotation; Violet was excluded arbitrarily, without having looked at her data, simply because we were aiming at a gender-balanced corpus (two female subjects and two male subjects).

  3. Choosing to use the Providence Corpus entails that we miss the earliest laughter episodes, which occur between the 3rd and 4th month of life (; Sroufe & Wunsch, 1972; Nwokah et al., 1994).

  4. For these two reasons we were forced on two occasions (Lily and Naima at time-point 5: 36 months) to integrate the analysis of the originally selected video with the temporally closest other video available, and sum the duration of minutes and laughs analysed (see Table 6). We selected the videos closest to our age of interest, but an important exception was made for Alex: the first video, which we analyze as related to the first time-point (12 months), was actually recorded at 16 months of age (the time when his parents reported he had a vocabulary of about 4 words).

  5. A detailed analysis of the annotations related to the objects of laughter and its pragmatic functions will be reported in a forthcoming work.

  6. Annotations and data are available at https://osf.io/48fmd/?view_only=a3bcf69ab71f43fdac820b1e46c6e5de.

  7. In the current work we consider children and mothers as two groups, leaving to a future work scrutiny over individual variabilities.

  8. The R scripts used raw annotations and data are available at https://osf.io/48fmd/?view_only=a3bcf69ab71f43fdac820b1e46c6e5de.

  9. The overall count of laughs in the corpus is based on the number of laughs identified by the first annotator.

  10. In Krippendorff (2012, p. 241) cut-off values of acceptability are proposed to guide a value interpretation, as rules of thumb: a ≥ 0.8 is considered as a reliable degree of agreement, while a ≥ 0.66 is considered as an acceptable value for tentative conclusions.

  11. A mixed-effect logistic model was not a viable option given the limited amount of data available.

  12. Only two laughs had a duration lower than 0.5 s, and they were not from the same video. There is, therefore, no possibility that two laughs occurred in the same window.

  13. The operationalization chosen is aimed at maximizing the informativity of our data, given the limited sample size.

  14. We acknowledge that the arousal annotation had a rather low degree of inter-annotator agreement and that our current analysis is based on the annotation agreed upon after discussion between annotators. Our results on this score, therefore, need to be viewed with caution.

  15. We decided to perform a McNemar’s Chi-squared test since it is a good non-parametric alternative to the Pearson’s Chi-squared test, and is a better fit for small paired nominal data samples (2 × 2 contingency table) compared to the Fisher’s Exact Test.

  16. For the analysis of duration, given the availability of sufficiently many data points (287 laughs), we chose to use ANOVA since it is a robust test for comparing a continuous variable such as duration among two groups and three conditions and testing their interaction.

  17. See footnote 15 for a justification of our choice.

  18. We performed a Fisher’s exact test, since it is a good alternative to Chi-square tests when dealing with small sample size. We could not perform a McNemar’s test, even though it would have been a more suitable option for paired data, since it can be applied only for 2 × 2 contingency tables (therefore viable for the 2 × 2 comparison between mothers and children overall, but not for the 2 × 5 comparison of speech laughter production over time for each participant).

  19. In the class antiphonal laughter, we include both laughter with no overlap (reciprocal in Nwokah et al., 1994) and those resulting in overlap (coactive in Nwokah et al., 1994).

  20. The choice to collapse points of observation was motivated by the fact that having a relatively small sample size considering each time-point independently would have resulted in a weak statistical reliability.

  21. This test was used here since it is a viable non-parametric alternative (appropriate for our data) to the paired Student’s t-test.

  22. We need to acknowledge again that the arousal annotation had a rather low degree of inter-annotator agreement and that our current analysis is based on the annotation agreed upon after discussion between annotators.

References

  • Addyman, C., Fogelquist, C., Levakova, L., & Rees, S. (2018). Social facilitation of laughter and smiles in preschool children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1048.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Apte, M. L. (1985). Humor and laughter: An anthropological approach. Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (1988). Social cognition and social interaction. The Psychologist, 1(5), 177–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asada, M. (2016). Modeling early vocal development through infant–caregiver interaction: A review. IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems, 8(2), 128–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bachorowski, J.-A., & Owren, M. J. (2001). Not all laughs are alike: Voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect. Psychological Science, 12(3), 252–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(2), 113–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.

  • Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of meaning: An integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 19(2), 163–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekinschtein, T. A., Davis, M. H., Rodd, J. M., & Owen, A. M. (2011). Why clowns taste funny: The relationship between humor and semantic ambiguity. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(26), 9665–9671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, K., D’Odorico, L., & Beaumont, S. (1993). Adult preferences for syllabic vocalizations: Generalizations to parity and native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(1), 109–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., & Manian, N. (2013). Maternal responsiveness and sensitivity reconsidered: Some is more. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt1), 957–971.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: Longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 867.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss v. 3 (Vol. 1). Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., & McLean, J. F. (2010). Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(9), 2355–2368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., McLean, J. F., & Brown, A. (2011). The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogs with humans and computers. Cognition, 121(1), 41–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgett, D. J., Laake, L. M., Gartstein, M. A., & Dorn, D. (2013). Development of infant positive emotionality: The contribution of maternal characteristics and effects on subsequent parenting. Infant and Child Development, 22(4), 362–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brugman, H., & Russel, A. (2004). Annotating multi-media/ multi-modal resources with ELAN. In Proceedings of the fourth international conference on language resources and evaluation (LREC’04), Lisbon, Portugal. European Language Resources Association (ELRA).

  • Buckley, B. (2012). Children’s communication skills: From birth to five years. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Camaioni, L. (1992). Mind knowledge in infancy: The emergence of intentional communication. Early Development and Parenting, 1(1), 15–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, i–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, A. J. (1973). Social facilitation of laughter in children. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9(6), 528–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, R. S. (2000). Children’s language learning: An interactionist perspective. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(1), 33–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L. (2013). The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 285–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1987). Mother–infant face-to-face interaction: The sequence of dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9 months. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collis, G. M. (1985). On the origins of turn-taking: Alternation and meaning. In Children’s Single-word Speech (pp. 217–230).

  • Cosentino, S., Sessa, S., & Takanishi, A. (2016). Quantitative laughter detection, measurement, and classification—a critical survey. IEEE Reviews in Biomedical Engineering, 9, 148–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cuffari, E. C., Di Paolo, E., & De Jaegher, H. (2015). From participatory sense-making to language: There and back again. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 1089–1125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, W., McKeown, G. J., Rychlowska, M., André, E., Wagner, J., & Lingenfelser, F. (2017). Social context disambiguates the interpretation of laughter. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: Using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56(3), 391–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davila Ross, M., Menzler, S., & Zimmermann, E. (2007). Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play. Biology Letters, 4(1), 27–30.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Davila-Ross, M., Allcock, B., Thomas, C., & Bard, K. A. (2011). Aping expressions? chimpanzees produce distinct laugh types when responding to laughter of others. Emotion, 11(5), 1013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demuth, K., Culbertson, J., & Alter, J. (2006). Word-minimality, epenthesis and coda licensing in the early acquisition of English. Language and Speech, 49(2), 137–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Denby, J., & Yurovsky, D. (2019). Parents’ linguistic alignment predicts children’s language development. In Proceedings of the 41th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1627–1632).

  • Devillers, L., & Vidrascu, L. (2007). Positive and negative emotional states behind the laughs in spontaneous spoken dialogs. In Interdisciplinary workshop on the phonetics of laughter (p. 37).

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial cues. Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, R., & Grimm, R. (2014). Quantifying categorical and conceptual convergence in child-adult dialogue. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society (Vol. 36).

  • Fogel, A. (1990). The process of developmental change in infant communicative action: Using dynamic systems theory to study individual ontogenies. Individual Differences in infancy: Reliability, Stability, Prediction, 341–358.

  • Fogel, A. (1977). Temporal organization in mother-infant face-to-face interaction. Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction, 119, 151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. (1982). Social play, positive affect, and coping skills in the first 6 months of life. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 2(3), 53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., Dickson, K. L., Hsu, H.-C., Messinger, D., Nelson-Goens, G. C., & Nwokah, E. (1997). Communication of smiling and laughter in mother-infant play: Research on emotion from a dynamic systems perspective. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1997(77), 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, J. R., Scherer, K. R., Roesch, E. B., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2007). The world of emotions is not two-dimensional. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1050–1057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Sociality of solitary smiling: Potentiation by an implicit audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, S., & Rathcke, T. (2018). Laugh is in the air? In Proceedings of laughter workshop 2018, Paris, France (pp. 21–24).

  • Fusaroli, R., Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., & Tylén, K. (2014). Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 147–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K. (2012). Carving language for social coordination: A dynamical approach. Interaction Studies, 13(1), 103–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginzburg, J., Mazzocconi, C., & Tian, Y. (2020). Laughter as language. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 8030–8035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child Development, 80(3), 636–644.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E. (2015). Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1167), 236–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, H. J., Aung, M. S., Romera-Paredes, B., McLoughlin, C., McKeown, G., Curran, W., & Bianchi-Berthouze, N. (2013). Laughter type recognition from whole body motion. In 2013 Humaine association conference on affective computing and intelligent interaction (pp. 349–355).

  • El Haddad, K., Cakmak, H., & Dutoit, T. (2018). On laughter intensity level: Analysis and estimation. In Proceedings of laughter workshop, Paris, France.

  • El Haddad, K., Chakravarthula, S. N., & Kennedy, J. (2019). Smile and laugh dynamics in naturalistic dyadic interactions: Intensity levels, sequences and roles. In 2019 International Conference on Multimodal Interaction (pp. 259–263).

  • Hilbrink, E. E., Gattis, M., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Early developmental changes in the timing of turn-taking: A longitudinal study of mother–infant interaction. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1492.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoicka, E., & Gattis, M. (2008). Do the wrong thing: How toddlers tell a joke from a mistake. Cognitive Development, 23(1), 180–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoicka, E., Jutsum, S., & Gattis, M. (2008). Humor, abstraction, and disbelief. Cognitive Science, 32(6), 985–1002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hough, J., Tian, Y., de Ruiter, L., Betz, S., Schlangen, D., & Ginzburg, J. (2016). Duel: A multi-lingual multimodal dialogue corpus for disfluency, exclamations and laughter. In 10th Edition of the language resources and evaluation conference.

  • Hsu, H.-C., & Fogel, A. (2001). Infant vocal development in a dynamic mother-infant communication system. Infancy, 2(1), 87–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, H.-C., & Fogel, A. (2003). Stability and transitions in mother-infant face-to-face communication during the first 6 months: A microhistorical approach. Developmental Psychology, 39(6), 1061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, H.-C., Nwokah, E. E., & Fogel, A. (1998). Characteristics of infant laughter during the first six months of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M. D., Rochat, P., & Stern, D. N. (2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i–149.

  • Kenderdine, M. (1931). Laughter in the pre-school child. Child Development, 2(3), 228–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, K. J. (2008). ‘Speech-smile’, ‘speech-laugh’, ‘laughter’and their sequencing in dialogic interaction. Phonetica, 65(1–2), 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kret, M. E., Venneker, D., Evans, B., Samara, I., & Sauter, D. (2021). The ontogeny of human laughter. Biology Letters, 17, 0319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krippendorff, K. (2012). Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology. Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ku, L.-C., Feng, Y.-J., Chan, Y.-C., Wu, C.-L., & Chen, H.-C. (2017). A re-visit of three-stage humor processing with readers’ surprise, comprehension, and funniness ratings: An erp study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 42, 49–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonardi, G., Nomikou, I., Rohlfing, K. J., & Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. (2016). Vocal interactions at the dawn of communication: the emergence of mutuality and complementarity in mother-infant interaction. In 2016 Joint IEEE international conference on development and learning and epigenetic robotics (ICDL-EPIROB) (pp. 288–293).

  • Lücking, A., Ptock, S., & Bergmann, K. (2011). Assessing agreement on segmentations by means of staccato, the segmentation agreement calculator according to thomann. In International gesture workshop (pp. 129–138).

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The childes project: Tools for analyzing talk: Volume i: Transcription format and programs, volume ii: The database. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: The socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Development, 991–1003.

  • Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C., Tesman, J. R., Shepard, B., Fogel, A., Reimers, M., & Zivin, G. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i–136.

  • Martin, R. A. (2010). The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayo, O., & Gordon, I. (2020). In and out of synchrony—behavioral and physiological dynamics of dyadic interpersonal coordination. Psychophysiology, 57(6), e13574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzocconi, C., Tian, Y., & Ginzburg, J. (2016). Multi-layered analysis of laughter. In Proceedings of SemDial.

  • Mazzocconi, C., Maraev, V., & Ginzburg, J. (2018). Laughter repair. In Proceedings of Demdial 2018 (aixdial), the 22nd workshop on the semantics and pragmatics of dialogue, Aix-en-Provence ,, France.

  • Mazzocconi, C., Maraev, V., Somashekarappa, V., & Howes, C. (2021). Looking for laughs: Gaze interaction with laughter pragmatics and coordination. In Proceedings of the 2021 international conference on multimodal interaction (pp. 636–644).

  • Mazzocconi, C., Tian, Y., & Ginzburg, J. (2020). What’s your laughter doing there? A taxonomy of the pragmatic functions of laughter. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 2020, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGhee, P. E. (1977). A model of the origins and early development of incongruity-based humour. In It’s a funny thing, humour (pp. 27–36). Elsevier.

  • McGhee, P. E. (1979). Humor: Its origin and development. WH Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeown, G., & Curran, W. (2015). The relationship between laughter intensity and perceived humour. In The 4th interdisciplinary workshop on laughter and other non-verbal vocalisations in speech, Enschede, Netherlands (pp. 27–29).

  • Mireault, G. C., & Reddy, V. (2016). Humor in infants: Developmental and psychological perspectives. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Misiek, T., Favre, B., & Fourtassi, A. (2020). Development of multi-level linguistic alignment in child-adult conversations. In Proceedings of the workshop on cognitive modeling and computational linguistics (pp. 54–58).

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, J. K. (2012). What made Freud laugh: An attachment perspective on laughter. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nomikou, I. (2015). The collaborative construction of early multimodal input and its significance for language development. Unpublished Doctoral Thesis, Bielefeld University.

  • Nomikou, I., Rohlfing, K. J., & Szufnarowska, J. (2013). Educating attention: Recruiting, maintaining, and framing eye contact in early natural mother–infant interactions. Interaction Studies, 14(2), 240–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, M. R., & Drummond, S. S. (1998). Communicative functions of laughter in aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 11(4), 391–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nwokah, E. E., Hsu, H.-C., Davies, P., & Fogel, A. (1999). The integration of laughter and speech in vocal communicationa dynamic systems perspective. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(4), 880–894.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nwokah, E. E., Hsu, H.-C., Dobrowolska, O., & Fogel, A. (1994). The development of laughter in mother-infant communication: Timing parameters and temporal sequences. Infant Behavior and Development, 17(1), 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, D. C., & Kowal, S. (2005). Laughter in Bill Clinton’s My Life (2004) interviews. Pragmatics: Quarterly Publication of the International Pragmatics Association (IPrA), 15(2), 275–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owren, M. J., & Bachorowski, J.-A. (2003). Reconsidering the evolution of nonlinguistic communication: The case of laughter. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(3), 183–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papafragou, A., Cassidy, K., & Gleitman, L. (2007). When we think about thinking: The acquisition of belief verbs. Cognition, 105(1), 125–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1985). The evolution of a functional model of nonverbal exchange: A personal perspective. Sequence and Pattern in Communicative Behavior, 190–205.

  • Patterson, M. L. (1976). An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy. Psychological Review, 83(3), 235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedroso, F. (2008). Reflexes. In M. M. Haith & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development (pp. 11–23). San Diego: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012370877-9.00133-X

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1945). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2006). Alignment as the basis for successful communication. Research on Language and Computation, 4(2–3), 203–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polimeni, J., & Reiss, J. P. (2006). Humor perception deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 141(2), 229–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proyer, R. T., & Ruch, W. (2010). Enjoying and fearing laughter: Personality characteristics of gelotophobes, gelotophiles, and katagelasticists. Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, 52(2), 148–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., Nomikou, I., & Rohlfing, K. J. (2013). Young children’s dialogical actions: The beginnings of purposeful intersubjectivity. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5(3), 210–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2004). Young children know that trying is not pretending: A test of the" behaving-as-if" construal of children’s early concept of pretense. Developmental Psychology, 40(3), 388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rauchbauer, B., & Grosbras, M.-H. (2020). Developmental trajectory of interpersonal motor alignment: Positive social effects and link to social cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 411–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (1991). Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Playing with others’ expectations: Teasing and mucking about in the first year. Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2001). Infant clowns: The interpersonal creation of humour in infancy. Enfance, 53(3), 247–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V., Williams, E., & Vaughan, A. (2002). Sharing humour and laughter in autism and down’s syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 93(2), 219–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reisenzein, R., Horstmann, G., & Schützwohl, A. (2019). The cognitive-evolutionary model of surprise: A review of the evidence. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(1), 50–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81(1), 6–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samson, A. C. (2013). Humor (lessness) elucidated–sense of humor in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: Review and introduction. Humor, 26(3), 393–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, D., Evans, B., Venneker, D., & Kret, M. (2018). How do babies laugh? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(3), 1840–1840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxton, M. (2009). The inevitability of child directed speech. In Language acquisition (pp. 62–86). Springer.

  • Smoski, M., & Bachorowski, J.-A. (2003). Antiphonal laughter between friends and strangers. Cognition & Emotion, 17(2), 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A., & Wunsch, J. P. (1972). The development of laughter in the first year of life. Child Development, 43, 1326–1344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steglich-Petersen, A., & Michael, J. (2015). Why desire reasoning is developmentally prior to belief reasoning. Mind & Language, 30(5), 526–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., & Bennett, S. L. (1975). Vocalizing in unison and in alternation: Two modes of communication within the mother-infant dyad. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 263(1), 89–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, M. B., Ver Hoeve, J. N., Roach, M. A., & Leavitt, L. A. (1986). The beginning of conversation: Early patterns of mother-infant vocal responsiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(4), 423–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Hopfield, N. (1995). How children tell a lie from a joke: The role of second-order mental state attributions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(2), 191–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Team, R. C. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Computer Software Manual.

  • Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educational Psychology Review, 3(4), 269–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., et al. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130).

  • Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R., & Carpenter, M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study. Primates, 35(2), 137–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. (2007). The neurobehavioral and social-emotional development of infants and children. WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trouvain, J. (2001). Phonetic aspects of “speech-laughs”. In Proceedings of the 2nd conference on orality and gestuality (pp. 634–639).

  • Trujillo, J. P., & Holler, J. (2021). The kinematics of social action: Visual signals provide cues for what interlocutors do in conversation. Brain Sciences, 11(8), 996.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Urbain, J., Niewiadomski, R., Mancini, M., Griffin, H., Cakmak, H., Ach, L., & Volpe, G. (2013). Multimodal analysis of laughter for an interactive system. In International conference on intelligent technologies for interactive entertainment (pp. 183–192).

  • Vettin, J., & Todt, D. (2004). Laughter in conversation: Features of occurrence and acoustic structure. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(2), 93–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warlaumont, A. S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., & Oller, D. K. (2014). A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1314–1324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Washburn, R. W., Sptiz, R. A., & Goodenough, F. L. (1972). Facial expression in children: Three studies. Ayer Company Pub.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkie, I., & Saxton, M. (2010). The origins of comic performance in adult-child interaction. Comedy Studies, 1(1), 21–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, P. T. (1937). Laughing and weeping, cheerfulness and depression: A study of moods among college students. The Journal of Social Psychology, 8(3), 311–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, R. D., & Frye, M. (1966). Some are laughing; some are not—Why? Psychological Reports, 18(3), 747–754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yurovsky, D., Doyle, G., & Frank, M. C. (2016). Linguistic input is tuned to children’s developmental level. In A. Papafragou, D. Grodner, D. Mirman, & J. C. Trueswell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th annual meeting of the cognitive science society, Cogsci 2016, Philadelphia, PA, August 10–13, 2016. cognitivesciencesociety.org.

  • Zigler, E., Levine, J., & Gould, L. (1966). Cognitive processes in the development of children’s appreciation of humor. Child Development, 37, 507–518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We owe heartfelt thanks to Yair Haendler for his help in the statistical analysis and to Véronique Pouillon and Dina Ginzburg for their help with interannotator agreement. We also would like to thank Andy Lucking, Mitja Nikolaus and Kevin El Haddad for their comments on a previous version of this paper. This work, carried out within the Labex BLRI (ANR-11-LABX-0036) and the Institut Convergence ILCB (ANR-16-CONV-0002), has benefited from support from the French government, managed by the French National Agency for Research (ANR) and the Excellence Initiative of Aix-Marseille University (A*MIDEX).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Both authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation and data analysis were performed by Chiara Mazzocconi. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Chiara Mazzocconi. Jonathan Ginzburg contributed to reviewing, commenting and editing on previous versions of the manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chiara Mazzocconi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A: Video Analyzed Details

The MLU (in morphemes) values reported are calculated over the full length of the videos partially annotated for laughter. These were computed using the MLU program in CLAN (MacWhinney, 2000). We excluded from the MLU calculation words that were unintelligible using the formula MLU +t*CHI –t%mor –syy -sxx @ for children and MLU +t*MOT –t%mor–syy -sxx @ for the mothers. We see that the children all have typical language development, with Naima standing out for her faster language development (Table 6).

Table 6 Information about videos analyzed—age, mean length of utterance, video-links, and transcripts

Appendix B: Laughter Annotation Protocol

Laughter Annotation Guidelines

Laughter Tier

  • Modality: audio and visual (facial expressions, head movement, body movements)

  • Criteria: laughter should differ from smile by the fact it contains either a body movement, a head movement or audible laughter related sounds.

  • Values: Laughter (L) or Speech-laughter (SL)

    • Laughter: Standalone laughter, i.e. laughter not overlapping with speech from the laugher her/himself

    • Speech-laughter: Laughter which overlaps with speech from the laugher her/himself.[When one speaks and laughs at the same time!]

  • Details:

  • Laughter: The segments start when an audio, facial expression or body movement event related to laughter is observed and stops when a breath intake is perceived whether audibly or visually (from the stomach, face, head, etc.). The breath intake is considered part of the laugh. If no breath intake is perceived the end of the segment is considered to be when the movement stops. In some cases breath intake sounds occurs after a relatively long delay. In this case, during this delay, if the participant is perceived as laughing than the breath intake marks the end of the laugh and is part of it. Otherwise the end of the laughter is the end of the sound or movement.

  • Speech-Laughter: The segments start when the laughter starts to happen and ends either with a breath intake sound or when the movement or sound ends. Speech-laughs can be as short as one vowel/consonant (30 ms) or as long as full sentences.

Arousal/Intensity Laughter Tier

  • Modality: audio and visual (facial expressions, head movement, body movements)

  • Values: low, medium, high

  • The arousal of the laughs are annotated subjectively based on the annotator’s perception.

  • !! Laughter level of arousal/intensity do not correspond to acoustic intensity! There can be silent laughs very high on arousal!!

Alignment Tier

  • Modality: audio and visual (facial expressions, head movement, body movements)

  • Values: Isolated (i), Antiphonal (A), Coactive onset (C)

    • Isolated: The laugh is not preceded by any other laugh from the partner (within 1 sec).

    • Antiphonal: A laugh that start shortly after the onset of the partner's laughter and within 1 sec after the partner’s laughter offset.

    • Coactive Onset: The interactants start laughing with the same onset time (considered same onset if the distance between the 2 laughs is less than 100 ms).

Response to Other’s Laughter Tier

  • Modality: audio and visual (facial expressions, head movement, body movements)

  • Every time a laughter is identified, look/listen at the multi-modal reaction from the partner.

  • Values: Explicit, Implicit, None, No_vis

    • Explicit: the partner responds to the laughter with a Laughter him/her self, a Smile, an orienting Look, an Exclamation or with a Clarification Request.

    • Implicit: after the laughter the partner just continues with her/his activity, e.g. child is singing and looking at the mother, the mother laughs, and the child continues singing and looking at the mother.

    • None: no orienting attention behavior can be observed, neither continuation of activity.

    • No_vis: the multimodal reaction is not visible, either because the participant is giving the back to the camera or is off-camera.

Appendix C: Supplemental Examples Extracted from the Providence Corpus (Demuth et al., 2006)

  1. (13)

    Example from Providence Corpus—Naima 010014 (263–267)—Mother laughter mimicry

    Mum: ring around the rosies

    Mum: a pocket full of posies

    Mum: ashes, ashes

    Mum: we all go down

    Child: <laughter/>

    Mum: <laughter/>

  1. (14)

    Example from Providence Corpus—Alex 030103 (398–401)—Child laughter mimicry

    Child: No this one!

    Mum: Alright! Can I use the pen?

    Child: No! [screaming]

    Mum: <laughter> Stop it! </laughter>

    Child: <laughter/>

    Mum: Stop that screaming!

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mazzocconi, C., Ginzburg, J. A Longitudinal Characterization of Typical Laughter Development in Mother–Child Interaction from 12 to 36 Months: Formal Features and Reciprocal Responsiveness. J Nonverbal Behav 46, 327–362 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00403-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00403-8

Keywords

Navigation