Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Implicit Reactions to Women in High Power Body Postures: Less Wonderful But Still Weaker

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

People rapidly form impressions of others based on their gender. Women tend to be liked more than men but men tend to be regarded as more powerful. However, a person’s nonverbal behavior has the potential to confirm or override these stereotypical impressions. Specifically, expansive, open body postures (e.g., based on widespread limbs) tend to convey high power compared to contracted, closed body positions. In three studies, we tested whether postural variations affected evaluations of men and women and impressions of their power. In Study 1, images of women elicited a more positive reactions than images of men in an affective misattribution procedure, but only when women enacted contracted body postures. In Studies 2 and 3, participants were slower to classify images of expansive postures as high power when enacted by women and slower to classify contracted postures as low power when enacted by men, but rated men and women similarly in explicit power-related judgments. Expansive body postures thus appeared to eliminate the usual positive reaction to women relative to men, but women still did not implicitly convey power to the same degree as men. Gender did not interfere with explicit, more controlled judgments of power. Together these studies demonstrate that gender implicitly interferes with perceptions of a person’s power, even in the presence of potentially individuating body postures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that throughout the manuscript d reflects the Cohen’s d that takes the correlation between the within-subjects variables into account. The pre-registration lists a slightly different power analysis based on conventional Cohen’s d (i.e., mean difference divided by the square root of the pooled variance).

References

  • Adams, R. B., Hess, U., & Kleck, R. E. (2015). The intersection of gender-related facial appearance and facial displays of emotion. Emotion Review,7(1), 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aidman, E. V., & Carroll, S. M. (2003). Implicit individual differences: Relationships between implicit self-esteem, gender identity, and gender attitudes. European Journal of Personality,17(1), 19–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury, MA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aries, E. (1996). Men and women in interaction: Reconsidering the differences. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, A. H., & Kelly, S. D. (2015). Picture power: Gender versus body language in perceived status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,39(4), 317–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bareket, O., Kahalon, R., Shnabel, N., & Glick, P. (2018). The madonna-whore dichotomy: Men who perceive women’s nurturance and sexuality as mutually exclusive endorse patriarchy and show lower relationship satisfaction. Sex Roles,79, 519–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Cohen, B. P., & Zelditch, M. (1972). Status characteristics and social interaction. American Sociological Review,37(3), 251–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M., & Manis, M. (1994). Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,66(1), 5–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In K. Srull & S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B., & Lui, L. N. (1989). The primacy of age and sex in the structure of person categories. Social Cognition,7(3), 262–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briton, N. J., & Hall, J. A. (1995). Beliefs about female and male nonverbal communication. Sex Roles,32(1–2), 79–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Incorporated.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carli, L. L., Loeber, C. C., & LaFleur, S. J. (1995). Nonverbal behavior, gender, and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,68(6), 1030–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J., & Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science,21(10), 1363–1368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., Hall, J. A., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Beliefs about the nonverbal expression of social power. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,29(2), 105–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin,112(1), 155–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, E. R. (2009). Intersectionality and research in psychology. American Psychologist,64(3), 170–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Contreras, J. M., Banaji, M. R., & Mitchell, J. P. (2013). Multivoxel patterns in fusiform face area differentiate faces by sex and race. PLoS ONE,8(7), e69684.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Correll, J., & Ridgeway, L. (2006). Expectation states theory. In J. Delamater (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 29–51). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic and Plenum Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology,1(1), 42–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crenshaw, K. (1991). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review,43(6), 1241–1299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., Zelazo, P. D., Packer, D. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2007). The iterative reprocessing model: A multilevel framework for attitudes and evaluation. Social Cognition,25(5), 736–760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Lemus, S., Spears, R., & Moya, M. (2012). The power of a smile to move you: Complementary submissiveness in women’s posture as a function of gender salience and facial expression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,38(11), 1480–1494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychological Bulletin,111(2), 203–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Brown, C. E., Heltman, K., Ellyson, S. L., & Keating, C. F. (1988). Power displays between women and men in discussions of gender-linked tasks: A multichannel study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,55(4), 580–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,82(1), 62–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,33(5), 510–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review,109(3), 573–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Mladinic, A. (1994). Are people prejudiced against women? Some answers from research on attitudes, gender stereotypes, and judgments of competence. European Review of Social Psychology,5(1), 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • England, P. (2010). The gender revolution: Uneven and stalled. Gender and Society,24(2), 149–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods,39(2), 175–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,23, 1–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2011). A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychological Review,118(2), 247–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky, A. D., Rucker, D. D., & Magee, J. C. (2015). Power: Past findings, present considerations, and future directions. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, J. A. Simpson, & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology: Interpersonal relationships (Vol. 3, pp. 421–460). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70(3), 491–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S. T., Eckes, T., Masser, B., Volpato, C., et al. (2004). Bad but bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men predict gender inequality in 16 nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,86(5), 713–728.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,97(1), 17–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hai, D. M., Khairullah, Z. Y., & Coulmas, N. (1982). Sex and the single armrest: Use of personal space during air travel. Psychological Reports,51(3), 743–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are a-changing or are they not? A comparison of gender stereotypes, 1983–2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly,40(3), 353–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Coats, E. J., & Smith LeBeau, L. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,131(6), 898–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Schmid Mast, M., & Latu, I. (2014). The vertical dimension of social relations and accurate interpersonal perception: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,39(2), 131–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatton, E., & Trautner, M. N. (2011). Equal opportunity objectification? The sexualization of men and women on the cover of rolling stone. Sexuality and Culture,15(3), 256–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M. (1995). Body politics revisited: What do we know today? In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships (pp. 27–61). New York, NY: Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M., & Harmon, S. (1985). The nonverbal semantics of power and gender: A perceptual study. In S. L. Ellyson & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Power, dominance, and nonverbal behavior (pp. 151–164). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2004). Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression. Emotion,4(4), 378–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, N. (2013). Conducting reaction time research in second language studies. New York, NY: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychological Bulletin,137(4), 616–642.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z., & Thagard, P. (1996). Forming impressions from stereotypes, traits, and behaviors: A parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory. Psychological Review,103(2), 284–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., Carroll, T. J., Karapetyan, A., Kaushik, N., et al. (2018). Relationship between the implicit association test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist,74(5), 569–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepore, M. (2014). 5 power poses you should start using at work now. Levo. https://www.levo.com/posts/5-power-poses-you-should-start-using-at-work-now.

  • Livingston, R. W., Rosette, A. S., & Washington, E. F. (2012). Can an agentic black woman get ahead? The impact of race and interpersonal dominance on perceptions of female leaders. Psychological Science,23(4), 354–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGinley, H., LeFevre, R., & McGinley, P. (1975). The influence of a communicator’s body position on opinion change in others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,31(4), 686–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monroe, B. M., Koenig, B. L., Wan, K. S., Laine, T., Gupta, S., & Ortony, A. (2018). Re-examining dominance of categories in impression formation: A test of dual-process models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,115(1), 1–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual differences in the desire for simpler structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,65(1), 113–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R., Furnham, A., Weis, L., Gee, M., Cardos, R., Lay, A., et al. (2016). NonVerbal presence: How changing your behaviour can increase your ratings for persuasion, leadership and confidence. Psychology,7, 488–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,89(3), 277–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, K., & Lundberg, K. (2014). The affect misattribution procedure: Ten years of evidence on reliability, validity, and mechanisms. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,8(12), 672–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelan, J. E., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Rudman, L. A. (2008). Competent yet out in the cold: Shifting criteria for hiring reflect backlash toward agentic women. Psychology of Women Quarterly,32(4), 406–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly,26(4), 269–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin,114(3), 510–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reimers, S., & Steward, N. (2015). Presentation and response timing accuracy in Adobe Flash and HTML5/JavaScript Web experiments. Behavior Research Methods,47(2), 309–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rennung, M., Blum, J., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). To strike a pose: No stereotype backlash for power posing women. Frontiers in Psychology,7, 1463.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ridgeway, C. L. (1987). Nonverbal behavior, dominance, and the basis of status in task groups. American Sociological Review,52, 683–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, R. S., Jussim, L., & Stevens, S. T. (2018). Reliance on individuating information and stereotypes in implicit and explicit person perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,75, 54–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic in-group bias: Why do women like women more than men like men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,87(4), 494–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., & Kilianski, S. E. (2000). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward female authority. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,26(11), 1315–1328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Nauts, S. (2012). Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,48(1), 165–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid Mast, M., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Who is the boss and who is not? Accuracy of judging status. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,28(3), 145–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sczesny, S. (2003). A closer look beneath the surface: Various facets of the think-manager-think-male stereotype. Sex Roles,49, 353–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F. (2010). The irrepressible communicative power of pride.(Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada.

  • Shariff, A. F., Tracy, J. L., & Markusoff, J. L. (2012). (Implicitly) judging a book by its cover: The power of pride and shame expressions in shaping judgments of social status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,38(9), 1178–1193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, J. (2012). A 21 word solution. Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract=2160588 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2160588.

  • Son Hing, L. S., Chung-Yan, G. A., Hamilton, L. K., & Zanna, M. P. (2008). A two-dimensional model that employs explicit and implicit attitudes to characterize prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,94(6), 971–987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stangor, C., Lynch, L., Duan, C., & Glas, B. (1992). Categorization of individuals on the basis of multiple social features. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,62(2), 207–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology,18(6), 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z., & Fragale, A. R. (2003). Power moves: Complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,84(3), 558–568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). Women in the workforce. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/newsroom/pdf/women_workforce_slides.pdf.

  • U.S. Department of Labor. (2010). Women in the labor force in 2010. Retrieved from http://www.dol.gov/wb/factsheets/Qf-laborforce-10.htm.

  • Visscher, P. M. (2008). Sizing up human height variation. Nature Genetics,40(5), 489–490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J., & Tiedens, Z. (2016). The subtle suspension of backlash: A meta-analysis of penalties for women’s implicit and explicit dominance behavior. Psychological Bulletin,142(2), 165–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S., Tiedens, L. Z., & Neale, M. (2015). The benefits of dominance complementarity in negotiations. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research,8(3), 194–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S. S. (2009). The benefits of dominance complementarity in negotiations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, Palo Alto, CA, USA.

  • Yang, P. (2010). Nonverbal gender differences: examining gestures of university-educated Mandarin Chinese speakers. Text & Talk,30(3), 333–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakrisson, I. (2005). Construction of a short version of the right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Personality and Individual Differences,39(5), 863–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to April H. Bailey.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Pilot Study to Study 1

Here we report the details and results of the pilot study to Study 1 reported in the main text. Pre-registered hypotheses, exclusion criteria, methods, and analysis plan for Study 1 were based on this pilot study.

Method

Participants

We sampled 126 respondents and excluded 11 participants whose average response on the rapid affect measure was below 200 ms (M = 105.69, SD = 42.91) and 19 participants who failed participant validity checks by indicating, for instance, that they were two different ages when asked at the beginning and end of the survey. The remaining 96 participants’ (56 women, 71 white, Mage = 38.68 SDage = 14.02) had plausible reaction times (RTs, M = 746.63, SD = 221.96), and as all factors were within-subjects, exclusions did not differ among conditions.

Measures and Procedure

Participants completed an AMP identical to that described in Study 1 in the main text.

Results

Each participant’s AMP score was the proportion of trials marked “pleasant” (vs. “unpleasant”). Trials with RTs less than 200 ms were omitted, which resulted in the omission of 2.36% (SD = 9.84%) of trials on average.

AMP scores were analyzed in a 2 (prime gender: male, female) × 2 (prime posture: contracted, expansive) repeated measures analysis of variance (rANOVA). Prime gender interacted with prime posture, F(1, 95) = 5.99, p = 0.016, η 2p  = 0.06. Specifically, participants reacted more positively to female primes (M = 0.63, SD = 0.20) than male primes (M = 0.58, SD = 0.23) but only in the contracted posture trials, t(95) = 2.01, p = 0.048, d = 0.20, and showed no preference for female primes (M = 0.57, SD = 0.21) compared to male primes (M = 0.59, SD = 0.19) in the expansive posture trials, t(95) = 0.73, p = 0.467, d = 0.08. Further, participants had a more positive reaction to female primes in contracted postures compared to female primes in expansive postures, t(95) = 2.26, p = 0.026, d = 0.23, while reactions to male primes in expansive and contracted postures did not differ, t(95) = 0.49, p = 0.627, d = 0.05.

Appendix 2: Study 2 Additional Details

Here we report the details of additional memory questions included in Study 2. These measures were included at the very end of Study 2 for exploratory purposes, and thus could not affect the results reported in the main text.

Participants saw four images taken from the set of 48 used in the posture classification task, a man and a woman in an expansive and a contracted posture. These were intermixed with images of a man and a woman in the same postures, but who had not been included in the classification task. Participants were instructed to rate each image on a scale from definitely have not seen before (1) to definitively have seen before (7).

Appendix 3: Replication to Study 3

Here we report the details of a replication to Study 3.

Method

Participants

We sampled 203 participants and excluded 26 whose error rates exceeded 15.00% (M = 89.64%, SD = 80.09) and one who failed validity checks described in Study 1. The remaining 176 participants (68 women, 137 white, Mage = 33.06 SDage = 9.15) had relatively low error rates (M = 5.45%, SD = 3.88%), and exclusions did not differ among the four between-subjects posture and gender conditions for the leadership judgments, χ2 (3, N = 203) = 5.08, p = 0.166.

Measures and Procedure

The posture classification task and leadership judgments were identical to that used in Study 3 (leadership qualities, α = 0.95).

Results

Posture Classification Task

RTs were prepared and analyzed in the same way as in Studies 2 and 3. Replicating Studies 2 and 3, target posture and target gender interacted, F(1, 175) = 21.45, p < 0.001, η 2p  = 0.11. Again, participants were slower to indicate that expansive postures were high power when classifying female targets (M = 844.95, SD = 241.54) compared to male targets (M = 826.64, SD = 224.71), t(175) = 2.38, p = 0.018, d = 0.18, and participants were slower to indicate that contracted postures were low power when classifying a male target (M = 853.57, SD = 250.19) compared to a female target (M = 821.88, SD = 195.95), t(175) = 3.68, p < 0.001, d = 0.28

Leadership Judgments

Leadership, hiring, and pay were analyzed in the same way as in Study 3. Replicating Study 3, Participants rated targets in expansive postures higher than targets in contracted postures on all three measures regardless of the target’s gender: leadership qualities, (M = 5.58, SD = 0.81; vs. M = 3.24, SD =0.99), F(1, 172) = 295.87, p < 0.001, η 2p  = 0.63, pay (M = $69,100, SD = $35,400; versus, M = $53,900, SD =$22,700), F(1, 172) = 11.28, p = 0.001, η 2p  = 0.06, and hiring, (M = 4.74, SD = 1.29; vs. M = 3.41, SD = 1.39), F(1, 171) = 42.73, p < 0.001, η 2p  = 0.20.

Note that when testing for potential participant gender effects, participant gender interacted with candidate gender concerning hireability, F(1, 167) = 3.97, p = 0.048, η 2p  = 0.02. Female participates rated female candidates (M = 4.59, SD = 1.56) as more hirable than male candidates (M = 3.89, SD = 1.35), F(1, 167) = 4.63, p = 0.033, η 2p  = 0.02, while male participants displayed no preference. For the present purposes this finding only reiterates that concerning explicit judgments participants display willingness to rate women at least as suitable as men (if not more) for leadership.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bailey, A.H., Lambert, R. & LaFrance, M. Implicit Reactions to Women in High Power Body Postures: Less Wonderful But Still Weaker. J Nonverbal Behav 44, 329–350 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00327-w

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00327-w

Keywords

Navigation