Abstract
Basic emotions theory (BET) is the most popular and deeply rooted psychological theory of both emotion and the facial behavior held to express it. We review its Western foundations and the key developments in its evolution, focusing on its parsing of facial expressions into two kinds: biological, categorical, iconic, universal “facial expressions of emotion,” versus modified, culturally diverse versions of those iconic expressions due to intermediation by learned “display rules.” We suggest that this dichotomy and its many corollaries are oversimplified, and that many of BET’s recent modifications are inconsistent in ways that may render it impossible to test and immune to falsification. In contrast, we suggest that the behavioral ecology view of facial displays, as an externalist and functionalist approach, resolves the quandaries and contradictions embedded in BET’s precepts and extensions.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.


Notes
Throughout this chapter, we use facial behavior or facial display following the ethological tradition. On occasion, we use the term “expression” by convention, but without the implication that facial behaviors transmit any “content”.
Unless otherwise stated, whenever we use the term “emotion,” it denotes the construal of that term within that particular research. Thus, it should not be assumed that “emotion” as used in one study or line of research is the same construct, or refers to the same set of behaviors, used in the next.
LeBrun’s depictions of the passions lie at the heart of BET, and via their insinuation into Basic Emotion Theories, they became ubiquitous in psychology and neuroscience. Indeed, skeptical approaches to these iconic Western representations of the passions are regarded by some BET advocates as anti-Darwinian or non-evolutionary (Ekman 2017; Izard 1971; Tracy 2014).
Lamarckian use-inheritance was popular during Charles Darwin’s time, and Darwin invoked it throughout Expression. It is now considered untenable. Note that inheritance through usage was required for Lamarckian transmission, unlike epigenetic marking, an accepted mechanism for within-lifespan acquisition of heritable traits (Armstrong 2014).
In Tomkins’s theoretical hermeneutics, (1) the “affects” were the bodily responses and the expressions, (2) the awareness of affect was the “feeling,” and (3) the feeling embedded in the context of memories of prior similar feelings was the “emotion.” These distinctions largely dropped out of subsequent BET formulations, which regarded “basic emotions” as hermetic packages which might be modulated by drives or triggered by memories.
Tracy and Randles (2011) highlighted four BET “models” and invited their representative supporters to a special issue of Emotion Review dedicated to BET (Ekman and Cordaro 2011; Izard 2011; Levenson 2011; Panksepp and Watt 2011). Although Ekman, Izard, Levinson, and Panksepp proposed different theories of basic emotions, Ekman’s model remains the most comprehensive in its stipulations and predictions.
There is considerable hand-waving among BET theorists, and emotion researchers generally, about the multifactorial nature of emotion, along with the discounting provision that qualia are only one component. Because qualia are unverifiable, no measurable proxies have been substantiated, and alterations in the other averred components (overt movements, autonomic adjustments, neurally localized activities) are only weakly intercorrelated and do not appear to conform to standard emotion typologies (e.g., Durán et al. 2017). We find no fault with a “multifactorial” definition of anything, of course, providing that the definition sets out what factors are expected and when. No such specification has been forthcoming, lending weight to the interpretation that the “multifactorial” definition merely postpones definition.
The mechanism governing display rules mutated over the years. Originally, there were universals in the antecedent events that could trigger activation of the Facial Affect Program (Ekman 1984; Ekman and Cordaro 2011), and the Program’s outputs were intercepted and aligned with display rules before they could erupt on the face. The interception points were never delineated, but the pre-post interception expressions may “differ in the involvement of the pyramidal and nonpyramidal pathways” (Ekman et al. 1981, p. 102). Objections by appraisal theorists (Scherer and Wallbott 1994) were accommodated by allowing that cultural differences in displays might hold sway on the stimulus end, such that complex appraisals could differentially determine what combination of basic emotions was activated. Thus, for appraisal theorists the action of cultural display rules changed from an output mechanism that altered the production of expressions, to an input mechanism that selected the emotions elicited. This particular redefinition of display rules appears to inject further doubt into how one interprets the three conditions of the Japanese-American experiment, which predicated its test of display rules on the prior assumption of equal emotions (Fridlund 1994; Leys 2017).
We confine our discussion here to BET’s categorical “facial expressions of emotion.” Ekman and Friesen (1969b) provided examples of other kinds of facial behavior such as symbolic faces (e.g., stylized tongue protrusions for “yucks!,” tongues in cheeks for skepticism), movements that highlighted language (e.g., brow flashes to show momentary interest) or regulated conversation (e.g., frowns to indicate disapproval, brow flashes in turn-taking), and self-stimulatory behavior (e.g., lip-wiping). These protolinguistic and paralinguistic movements probably constitute the vast majority of everyday facial behavior. Chovil’s detailed observations of dyadic conversations suggested that fewer than 20% could be classified as one of BET’s categorical “expressions of emotion” (Chovil 1989, 1991). Likewise, Fridlund (1994, p. 312) suggested that many occurrences of those iconic BET faces may simply be paralinguistic interjections.
Apart from repeating Ekman’s incomplete account, Matsumoto and Hwang’s (2013) quote illustrates the common conflation of “emotions” and “expressions” within BET: What can be masked within BET is not the emotions but the expressions of those emotions.
Western commentators on the Neurocultural Model and its origins in Tomkins’s and prior categorical views on emotion often do not indicate just how Western the models are, when they: (1) reductively neurologize emotion and make its “essence” a collection of neural circuits, and (2) make emotion intracorporeal and thus subpersonal. Different ideas about “emotion” that are not reductive or subpersonal abound in other cultures and even in the history of Western conceptions of emotion, and non-reductive emotion accounts have emerged in contemporary psychology (e.g., Barrett 2017; Russell 2009). Given the ineffability and intractability of modern emotion concepts, one is hard-put to be presentist and claim progress. Lang (2018), Matt (2011), Reddy (2009), Rosenwein (2014) and especially Plamper (2015) have reviewed “turns” in scholarly treatments of emotion and how it has been: (a) experienced (originally in largely moral and/or theological terms), (b) expressed (as a function of race, religion, social class, gender roles, family structure and child-rearing practices, and a host of other factors), (c) localized (in neurology, as adduced from ritual actions or other culturally situated action trajectories, or as an emergent process interstitial among cultural members), and (d) judged (dangerous, unruly, or advantageous). As Stearns and Stearns (1985) stated in their classic paper on “emotionology,” “No longer can we assume without proof … that people in the past shared our emotional experience, that we can use contemporary psychology to elucidate past behavior, or that we can use past data, without careful analysis, to bolster contemporary psychological theory” (p. 820).
Al-Shawaf et al. (2016) do not account for how “emotions” could be products of natural selection unless they were causal entities that produced behavior benefitting reproductive fitness, directly or indirectly. Natural selection works on proximal effects and not interior causes, and so the evolution of specific “emotions” would imply selection of behaviors specifically attributable to those emotions. BET (pre-1992) was not adaptationist given that it embraced Darwin’s reflexive, vestigial view. The ev-psych framework (post-1992), however, emphasized adaptive, strategic movements, but not necessarily automatic “expressions of emotions.” Indeed, much of evolutionary game theory had already suggested the tactical benefits of non-expression (e.g., Krebs and Dawkins 1984).
Previous treatments of BECV presented facial displays from the emitter’s standpoint, as “declarations of intent,” or indications of “social motives” (e.g., Fridlund 1994, 1997, 2017a). Here (Table 1) we present common usages of those facial displays. We made this shift because, despite our repeated statement that “intentions” and “motives” in BECV were external and to be adduced from the emitter’s line of action, many readers insisted on treating them as subpersonal, psychological states that were interchangeable with, or part of, emotion. The alternative presentations are equivalent because the likeliest scenario for the evolution of signals like facial displays is that they arose biologically via the ritualization, or culturally via the conventionalization, of Heinroth’s and Huxley’s “intention movements.” These movements were incipient versions of social actions (Fridlund 1994; Smith 1977).
References
Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2016). Human emotions: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Emotion Review, 8(2), 173–186.
Armstrong, L. (2014). Epigenetics. New York, NY: Garland Science, Taylor & Francis.
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338(6111), 1225–1229.
Bąk, H. K. (2016). Emotional prosody processing for non-native English speakers. Basel, Switzerland: Springer.
Barrett, L. F. (2017). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1–23.
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 286–290.
Beck, J. (2015, February 24). Hard feelings: Science’s struggle to define emotions. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2015/02/hard-feelings-sciences-struggle-to-define-emotions/385711.
Bell, C. (1806). Essays on the anatomy of expression in painting. London: Longman.
Bender, A., Hutchins, E., & Medin, D. L. (2010). Anthropology in cognitive science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 374–385.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Breugelmans, S. M., Chasiotis, D. L., & Sam, A. (2011). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications (3rd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and context. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Boag, S. (2011). Explanation in personality psychology: “Verbal magic” and the five-factor model. Philosophical Psychology, 24(2), 223–243.
Boccagni, P., & Baldassar, L. (2015). Emotions on the move: Mapping the emergent field of emotion and migration. Emotion, Space, and Society, 16, 73–80.
Browne, J. (1985). Darwin and the expression of the emotions. In D. Kohn (Ed.), The Darwinian heritage (pp. 307–326). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York, NY: Guilford.
Burkhardt, R. W. (1985). Darwin on animal behavior and evolution. In D. Kohn (Ed.), The Darwinian heritage (pp. 327–365). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Calder, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2005). Understanding the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 641–651.
Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 205–218.
Chovil, N. (1989). Communicative functions of facial displays in conversation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Victoria.
Chovil, N. (1991). Communicative functions of facial displays. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 141–154.
Colombetti, G. (2014). The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2017). Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 114, E7900–E7909.
Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2018). Facial displays are tools for social influence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(5), 388–399.
Crivelli, C., & Gendron, M. (2017). Facial expressions and emotions in indigenous societies. In J. M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 497–515). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Crivelli, C., Jarillo, S., & Fridlund, A. J. (2016a). A multidisciplinary approach to research in small-scale societies: Emotions and facial expressions in the field. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1073.
Crivelli, C., Jarillo, S., Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2016b). Reading emotions from faces in two indigenous societies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(7), 830–843.
Crivelli, C., Russell, J. A., Jarillo, S., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2016c). The fear gasping face as a threat display in a Melanesian society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 113(44), 12403–12407.
Crivelli, C., Russell, J. A., Jarillo, S., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2017). Recognizing spontaneous facial expressions of emotion in a small-scale society in Papua New Guinea. Emotion, 17(2), 337–347.
Crivelli, C., Jarillo, S., Fernández-Dols, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (2018). Links among situations, emotions, and facial displays in a Melanesian small-scale society. Manuscript under review.
Dalai Lama, T. G., & Ekman, P. (2008). Emotional awareness: Overcoming the obstacles to psychological balance and compassion: A conversation between the Dalai Lama and Paul Ekman. New York, NY: Times Books.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London, UK: Murray.
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872).
Descartes, R. (2015). The passions of the soul and other late philosophical writings (M. Moriarty, Trans.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1649).
Du, S., Tao, Y., & Martinez, A. M. (2014). Compound facial expressions of emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 111(15), 1554–1662.
Durán, J. I., Reisenzein, R., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2017). Coherence between emotions and facial expressions. In J. M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 107–129). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ekman, P. (1972). Universal and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. R. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1971 (Vol. 19, pp. 207–283). Lincoln, NE: Nebraska University Press.
Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Ekman, P. (1980). The face of man: Expressions of universal emotions in a New Guinea village. New York, NY: Garland.
Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the human face (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–344). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York, NY: Norton.
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169–200.
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 384–392.
Ekman, P. (1997). Expression or communication about emotion. In N. L. Segal, G. E. Weisfeld, & C. C. Weisfeld (Eds.), Uniting biology and psychology: Integrated perspectives on human development (pp. 315–338). Washington, DC: APA Press.
Ekman, P. (2016). What scientists who study emotion agree about. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 11(1), 31–34.
Ekman, P. (2017). Facial expressions. In J. M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 39–56). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ekman, P., & Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emotion Review, 3(4), 364–370.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969a). Nonverbal behavior and cues to deception. Psychiatry, 32, 88–106.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969b). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1982). Felt, false, and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 238–252.
Ekman, P., & O’Sullivan, M. (1987). The role of context in interpreting facial expression: Comment on Russell and Fehr. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 86–88.
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotions. Science, 164, 86–88.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). What are the similarities and differences in facial behavior across cultures? In P. Ekman, W. V. Friesen, & P. Ellsworth (Eds.), Emotion in the human face (pp. 128–143). New York, NY: Pergamon Press.
Ekman, P., Hager, J. C., & Friesen, W. V. (1981). The symmetry of emotional and deliberate facial actions. Psychophysiology, 18, 101–105.
Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353.
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203–235.
Fernández-Dols, J. M. (1999). Facial expression and emotion: A situational perspective. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 242–261). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2017). Natural facial behaviour: A view from psychological constructionism and pragmatics. In J. M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 77–92). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Fernández-Dols, J. M., & Carrera, C. (2011). Le bon dieu est dans le detail: Is smiling the recognition of happiness? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(6), 446–448.
Fernández-Dols, J. M., & Carroll, J. M. (1997). Is the meaning perceived in facial expression independent of its context? In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernández-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 275–294). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fernández-Dols, J. M., & Crivelli, C. (2013). Emotion and expression: Naturalistic studies. Emotion Review, 5(1), 24–29.
Fischer, J. (2017). Monkeytalk: Inside the worlds and minds of primates. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Fridlund, A. J. (1991a). Evolution and facial action in reflex, social motive, and paralanguage. Biological Psychology, 32(1), 3–100.
Fridlund, A. J. (1991b). Sociality of solitary smiling: Potentiation by an implicit audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 229–240.
Fridlund, A. J. (1992a). The behavioral ecology and sociality of human faces. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 90–121). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Fridlund, A. J. (1992b). Darwin’s anti-Darwinism in The Expression of the emotions in man and animals. In K. Strongman (Ed.), International review of emotion (Vol. 2, pp. 117–137). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Fridlund, A. J. (1997). The new ethology of human facial expressions. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernández-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 103–129). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fridlund, A. J. (2017a). The behavioral ecology view of facial displays, 25 years later. In J. M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 77–92). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Fridlund, A. J. (2017b). On scorched earths and bad births: Scarantino’s misbegotten “theory of affective pragmatics”. Psychological Inquiry, 28(2–3), 197–205.
Fridlund, A. J. (2018, April). What can audience effects teach us about facial displays? Paper presented at the Consortium of European Research on Emotion Conference (CERE), Glasgow.
Fridlund, A. J., Sabini, J. P., Hedlund, L. E., Schaut, J. A., Shenker, J. I., & Knauer, M. J. (1990). Social determinants of facial expressions during affective imagery: Displaying to the people in your head. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14, 113–137.
Fridlund, A. J., Kenworthy, K., & Jaffey, A. K. (1992). Audience effects in affective imagery: Replication and extension to dysphoric imagery. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16, 191–212.
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural differences in facial expressions in a social situation: An experimental test of the concept of display rules. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California, San Francisco.
Gendron, M., Roberson, D., van der Vyver, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2014). Perceptions of emotion from facial expressions are not culturally universal: Evidence from a remote culture. Emotion, 14(2), 251–262.
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in making meaning of facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 211–219.
Gilovich, T., Keltner, D., Chen, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2016). Social psychology (4th ed.). New York, NY: Norton.
Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and facial expression: Open-ended methods find more expressions and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 13(3), 225–266.
Hassin, R. R., Aviezer, H., & Bentin, D. (2013). Inherently ambiguous: Facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emotion Review, 5(1), 60–65.
Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A. (1995). The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective state and social situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 280–288.
Hogg, C. (2014). Subject of passions: Charles Le Brun and the emotions of absolutism. Psychological Quarterly, 93, 65–94.
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Izard, C. E. (2011). Forms and functions of emotions: Matters of emotion–cognition interactions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 371–378.
Jack, R. E., & Schyns, P. G. (2017). Toward a social psychophysics of face communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 269–297.
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 109, 7241–7244.
Jack, R. E., Crivelli, C., & Wheatley, T. (2018). Data-driven methods to diversify knowledge of human psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 1–5.
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205.
Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. W. (1991). An audience effect on smile production in 10-months-old infants. Psychological Science, 2, 45–49.
Kagan, J. (2007). What is emotion?. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Keltner, D. (2009). Born to be good: The science of a meaningful life. New York, NY: Norton.
Keltner, D., & Cordaro, D. T. (2017). Understanding multimodal emotional expressions: Recent advances in Basic Emotion Theory. In J. M. Fernández-Dols & J. A. Russell (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 57–75). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Keltner, D., & Ekman, P. (2000). Facial expression of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 236–249). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Keltner, D., & Shiota, M. N. (2003). New displays and new emotions: A commentary on Rozin and Cohen (2003). Emotion, 3, 86–91.
Keltner, D., Marsh, J., & Smith, J. A. (Eds.). (2010). The compassionate instinct: The science of human goodness. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Keltner, D., Tracy, J., Sauter, D., Cordaro, D. T., & McNeil, G. (2016). Expression of emotion. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 467–482). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5(4), 345–379.
Konstan, D. (2006). The emotions of the Ancient Greeks: Studies in Aristotle and classical literature. London, UK: University of Toronto Press.
Koopman-Holm, B., & Matsumoto, D. (2011). Values and display rules for specific emotions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(3), 355–371.
Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology (2nd ed., pp. 380–402). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Landis, C. (1924). Studies of emotional reactions II: General behavior and facial expression. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 4, 447–509.
Lang, J. (2018). New histories of emotion. History and Theory, 57, 104–120.
Langfeld, H. S. (1917). The judgment of emotions from facial expressions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 13, 172–184.
Levenson, R. W. (2011). Basic emotions questions. Emotion Review, 3(4), 379–386.
Leys, R. (2010). How did fear become a scientific object and what kind of object is it? Representations, 110, 66–104.
Leys, R. (2017). The ascent of affect: Genealogy and critique. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Lorenz, K. Z. (1970). Studies on animal and human behavior (Vol. 1 and 2). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mace, R., & Holden, C. J. (2005). A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 20(3), 116–121.
Marler, P. R., Duffy, A., & Pickert, R. (1986a). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken: I. Does a sender communicate information about the quality of a food referent to a receiver? Animal Behavior, 34, 188–193.
Marler, P. R., Duffy, A., & Pickert, R. (1986b). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken: II. Is a sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver? Animal Behavior, 34, 194–198.
Martin, G. N., & Carlson, N. R. (2019). Psychology (6th ed.). Harlow, UK: Pearson.
Martinez, A. M. (2017). Computational models of face perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(3), 263–269.
Matsumoto, D. (2001). Culture and emotion. In D. Matsumoto (Ed.), The handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 171–194). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2013). Cultural display rules. In K. D. Keith (Ed.), The encyclopedia of cross-cultural psychology (p. 303). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Matsumoto, D., & van de Vijver, F. (Eds.). (2011). Cross-cultural research methods in psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2009). Spontaneous facial expressions of emotion of congenitally and noncongenitally blind individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(1), 1–10.
Matsumoto, D., Consolacion, T., Yamada, H., Suzuki, R., Franklin, B., Paul, S., et al. (2002). American-Japanese cultural differences in judgements of emotional expressions of different intensities. Cognition and Emotion, 16(6), 721–747.
Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M. N., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. G. (2008). Facial expressions of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 211–234). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Matsumoto, D., Willingham, B., & Olide, A. (2009). Sequential dynamics of culturally moderated facial expressions of emotion. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1269–1275.
Matsumoto, D., Frank, M. G., & Hwang, H. S. (2013). Nonverbal communication: Science and applications. London, UK: SAGE.
Matt, S. J. (2011). Current emotion research in history: Or, doing history from the inside out. Emotion Review, 3, 117–124.
Medin, D. L., & Bang, M. (2014). Who’s asking? Native science, Western science and science education. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mendoza Straffon, L. (Ed.). (2016). Cultural phylogenetics. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Montagu, J. (1994). The expression of the passions. Yale, CT: Yale University Press.
Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Universality revisited. Emotion Review, 5(1), 8–15.
Niedenthal, P. M., & Ric, F. (2017). Psychology of emotion (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The simulation of smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expressions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(6), 417–433.
Niedenthal, P. M., Rychlowska, M., Wood, A., & Zhao, F. (2018). Heterogeneity of long-history migration predicts smiling, laughter and positive emotion across the globe and within the United States. PLoS ONE, 13(8), e0197651.
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97, 315–331.
Panksepp, J., & Watt, D. (2011). What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emotion Review, 3(4), 1–10.
Parkinson, B. (2005). Do facial movements express emotions or communicate motives? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(4), 278–311.
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and interpersonal processes. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Piderit, T. (1867). Wissenschaftliches system der mimik und physiognomik. [Scientific system of mimicry and physiognomy]. Detmold, Germany: Kingenberg.
Plamper, J. (2015). The history of emotions: An introduction. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Reddy, W. M. (2009). The navigation of feeling. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Reisenzein, R., Studtmann, M., & Horstmann, G. (2013). Coherence between emotion and facial expression. Emotion Review, 5(1), 16–23.
Richerson, P. J. (2019). An integrated Bayesian theory of phenotypic flexibility. Behavioural Processes. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.02.002.
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1992). Pygmalion in the classroom (Expanded ed.). New York, NY: Irvington.
Rosenwein, B. H. (2014). Modernity: A problematic category in the history of emotion. History and Theory, 53, 69–78.
Ross, S. (1984). Painting the passions: Charles LeBrun’s Conference sur l’expression. Journal of the History of Ideas, 45, 25–47.
Rozin, P., & Cohen, A. B. (2003). High frequency of facial expressions corresponding to confusion, concentration, and worry in an analysis of naturally occurring facial expressions of Americans. Emotion, 3, 68–75.
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 102–141.
Russell, J. A. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion: What lies beyond minimal universality? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 379–391.
Russell, J. A. (2009). Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 1259–1283.
Russell, J. A., Bachorowski, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2003). Facial and vocal expressions of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 329–349.
Rychlowska, M., Cañadas, E., Wood, A., Krumhuber, E. G., Fischer, A., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2014). Blocking mimicry makes true and false smiles look the same. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e90876.
Rychlowska, M., Miyamoto, Y., Matsumoto, D., Hess, U., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Kamble, S., et al. (2015). Heterogeneity of long-history migration explains cultural differences in reports of emotional expressivity and the functions of smiles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 112(19), 2429–2436.
Sauter, D. (2017). The nonverbal communication of positive emotions: An emotion family approach. Emotion Review, 9(3), 222–234.
Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2015). Emotional vocalizations are recognized across cultures regardless of the valence of distractors. Psychological Science, 26(3), 354–356.
Scarantino, A. (2017). How to do things with emotional expressions: The theory of affective pragmatics. Psychological Inquiry, 28(2–3), 165–185.
Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–328.
Schmidt, K. L., & Cohn, J. F. (2001). Human facial expressions as adaptations: Evolutionary questions in facial expression research. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 44, 3–24.
Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R. (2012). Facial expressions in response to a highly surprising event exceeding the field of vision: A test of Darwin’s theory of surprise. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(6), 657–664.
Shariff, A. F., & Tracy, J. L. (2011). What are emotion expressions for? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 395–399.
Shiota, M. N., & Kalat, J. W. (2017). Emotion (3rd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Shiota, M. N., Campos, B., & Keltner, D. (2003). The faces of positive emotion: prototype displays of awe, amusement, and pride. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000, 296–299.
Sihvola, J., & Engberg-Pedersen, T. (Eds.). (1998). The emotions in Hellenistic Philosophy. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior of communicating. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sng, O., Neuberg, S. L., Varnum, M. E. W., & Kenrisk, D. T. (2018). The behavioral ecology of cultural psychological variation. Psychological Review, 125(5), 714–743.
Snyder, P. J., Kaufman, R., Harrison, J., & Maruff, P. (2010). Charles Darwin’s emotional expression “experiment” and his contribution to modern neuropharmacology. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 19(2), 158–170.
Solomon, R. C. (2007). What is an emotion? (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Stearns, P. N., & Stearns, C. Z. (1985). Emotionology: Clarifying the history of emotions and emotional standards. American Historical Review, 90, 813–836.
Tinbergen, N. (1953). Social behavior in animals. London, UK: Chapman and Hall.
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. I: The positive affects. New York, NY: Springer.
Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. II: The negative affects. New York, NY: Springer.
Tomkins, S. S. (1991). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. III: The negative affects: Anger and fear. New York, NY: Springer.
Tomkins, S. S. (1992). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. IV: Cognition: Duplication and transformation of information. New York, NY: Springer.
Tomkins, S. S., & McCarter, R. (1964). What and where are the primary affects? Some evidence for a theory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 119–158.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375–424.
Tracy, J. L. (2014). An evolutionary approach to understanding distinct emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 308–312.
Tracy, J. L. (2016). Take pride: Why the deadliest sin holds the secret to human success. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin Harcourt.
Tracy, J. L., & Randles, D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: A review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3, 397–405.
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Show your pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion expression. Psychological Science, 15(3), 194–197.
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008). The nonverbal expressions of pride: Evidence for cross-cultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 516–530.
Tracy, J. L., Randles, D., & Steckler, C. M. (2015). The nonverbal communication of emotion. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 25–30.
U. S. Government Accountability Office. (2013). TSA should limit future funding for behavior detection activities (GAO Publication No. 14-158T). Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved at https://www.gao.gov/products/GAO-14-158T.
U. S. Government Accountability Office. (2018). Counterterrorism: DOD should fully address security assistance planning elements in global train and equip project proposals (GAO Publication No. 18-449). Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office. Retrieved at https://www.gao.gov/products/GAO-18-449.
Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Porter, S. (2011). Pitfalls and opportunities in nonverbal and verbal lie detection. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11(3), 89–121.
Wagner, H. L., & Smith, J. (1991). Facial expression in the presence of friends and strangers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 201–214.
Waller, B. M., Whitehouse, J., & Micheletta, J. (2018). Rethinking primate facial expression: A predictive framework. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 13–21.
Whiten, A., Ayala, F. J., Feldman, M. W., & Laland, K. N. (2017). The extension of biology through culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 114, 7775–7781.
Wundt, W. (1897). Outlines of psychology (C. H. Judd, Trans.). Oxford, UK: Engelman.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by a British Academy/Leverhulme Small Research Grant (SRG18R1-180740) awarded to Carlos Crivelli.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Crivelli, C., Fridlund, A.J. Inside-Out: From Basic Emotions Theory to the Behavioral Ecology View. J Nonverbal Behav 43, 161–194 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00294-2
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-019-00294-2