Abstract
Previous research has suggested that the ability to recognize vocal portrayals of socio-emotional expressions improves with age throughout childhood and adolescence. The current study examined whether stimulus-level factors (i.e., the age of the speaker and the type of expression being conveyed) interacted with listeners’ developmental stage to predict listeners’ recognition accuracy. We assessed mid-adolescent (n = 50, aged 13–15 years) and adult (n = 87, 18–30 years) listeners’ ability to recognize basic emotions and social expressions in the voices of both adult and youth actors. Adults’ emotional prosody was better recognized than that of youth, and adult listeners were more accurate overall than were mid-adolescents. Interaction effects revealed that youths’ accuracy was equivalent to adult listeners’ when hearing adult portrayals of anger, disgust, friendliness, happiness, and meanness, and youth portrayals of disgust, happiness, and meanness. Our findings highlight the importance of speaker characteristics and type of expression on listeners’ ability to recognize vocal cues of emotion and social intent.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Hu was computed as follows (Wagner 1993): (number of correct responses/number of times that stimuli category was presented) × (number of correct responses/number of times that response was made).
References
Allgood, R., & Heaton, P. (2015). Developmental change and cross-domain links in vocal and musical emotion recognition performance in childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 398–403. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12097.
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614.
Belin, P., Fillion-Bilodeau, S., & Gosselin, F. (2008). The Montreal affective voices: A validated set of nonverbal affect bursts for research on auditory affective processing. Behaviour Research Methods, 40, 531–539. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.2.531.
Blakemore, S.-J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 296–312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x.
Blasi, A., Mercure, E., Lloyd-Fox, S., Thomson, A., Brammer, M., Sauter, D., et al. (2011). Early specialization for voice and emotion processing in the infant brain. Current Biology, 21, 1220–1224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.009.
Boyatzis, C. J., & Satyaprasad, C. (1994). Children’s facial and gestural decoding and encoding: Relations between skills and with popularity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 37–55. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02169078.
Brosgole, L., & Weisman, J. (1995). Mood recognition across the ages. International Journal of Neuroscience, 82, 169–189. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459508999800.
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Cheang, H. S., & Pell, M. D. (2008). The sound of sarcasm. Speech Communication, 50, 366–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2007.11.003.
Chronaki, G., Hadwin, J. A., Garner, M., Maurage, P., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2015). The development of emotion recognition from facial expressions and non-linguistic vocalizations during childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 218–236. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12075.
Fitch, W. T., & Giedd, J. (1999). Morphology and development of the human vocal tract: A study using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 1511–1522.
Frank, M. G., & Stennett, J. (2001). The forced-choice paradigm and the perception of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 75–85.
Gallois, C., & Callan, V. J. (1986). Decoding emotional messages: Influence of ethnicity, sex, message type, and channel. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 755–762. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.4.755.
Grossmann, T. (2010). The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 219–236. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-2010-0499.
Grossmann, T., Oberecker, R., Koch, S. P., & Friederici, A. D. (2010). The developmental origins of voice processing in the brain. Neuron, 65, 852–858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.001.
Hawk, S. T., van Kleef, G. A., Fischer, A. H., & van der Schalk, J. (2009). “Worth a thousand words”: Absolute and relative decoding of nonlinguistic affect vocalizations. Emotion, 9, 293–305. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015178.
Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2000). Vocal communication of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), The handbook of emotion (pp. 220–235). New York: Guilford. ISBN-10: 1609180445.
Jürgens, R., Grass, A., Drolet, M., & Fischer, J. (2015). Effect of acting experience on emotion expression and recognition in voice: Non-actors provide better stimuli than expected. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39, 195–214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-015-0209-5.
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770–814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770.
Kilford, E. J., Garrett, E., & Blakemore, S. J. (2016). The development of social cognition in adolescence: An integrated perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 106–120.
Koo, T. K., & Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine, 15, 155–163.
LaFrance, M. (2002). Smile boycotts and other body politics. Feminism & Psychology, 12, 319–323. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959353502012003003.
Larson, R., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Daily companionship in late childhood and early adolescence: Changing developmental contexts. Child Development, 62, 284–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01531.x.
McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 424–453. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.424.
Merten, D. E. (1997). The meaning of meanness: Popularity, competition, and conflict among junior high school girls. Sociology of Education, 70, 175–191.
Mill, A., Allik, J., Realo, A., & Valk, R. (2009). Age-related differences in emotion recognition ability: A cross-sectional study. Emotion, 9, 619–630. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016562.
Mitchell, R. L. C., & Ross, E. D. (2013). Attitudinal prosody: What we know and directions for future study. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 471–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.027.
Morningstar, M., Dirks, M. A., & Huang, S. (2017a). Vocal cues underlying youth and adult portrayals of socio-emotional expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 41, 155–183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0250-7.
Morningstar, M., Dirks, M. A., Rappaport, B. I., Pine, D. S., & Nelson, E. E. (2017b). Associations between anxious and depressive symptoms and the recognition of vocal socio-emotional expressions in youth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1350963.
Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E. B., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704003915.
Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (2001). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.
Pell, M. D., Paulmann, S., Dara, C., Alasseri, A., & Kotz, S. A. (2009). Factors in the recognition of vocally expressed emotions: A comparison of four languages. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 417–435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2009.07.005.
Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38, 784–791. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784.
Riggio, R. E., & Friedman, H. S. (1986). Impression formation: The role of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 421–427. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.50.2.421.
Rothman, A. D., & Nowicki, S. (2004). A measure of the ability to identify emotion in children’s tone of voice. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 67–92.
Sauter, D. A., Panattoni, C., & Happé, F. (2012). Children’s recognition of emotions from vocal cues. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02081.x.
Scherer, K. R. (1996). Adding the affective dimension: A new look in speech analysis and synthesis. Proceedings of the 4th international conference of spoken language processing. Philadelphia, USA.
Scherer, K. R. (2003). Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms. Speech Communication, 40, 227–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00084-5.
Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., Wallbott, H. G., & Goldbeck, T. (1991). Vocal cues in emotion encoding and decoding. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 123–148. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00995674.
Simmons, R. (2002). Odd girl out: The hidden culture of aggression in girls. New York: Harcourt. ISBN 054735102X.
Spackman, M. P., Brown, B. L., & Otto, S. (2009). Do emotions have distinct vocal profiles? A study of idiographic patterns of expression. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1565–1588. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802536268.
Stanton-Salazar, R. D., & Spina, S. U. (2005). Adolescent peer networks as a context for social and emotional support. Youth & Society, 36, 379–417. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X04267814.
Thompson, A. E., & Voyer, D. (2014). Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 28, 1164–1195. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.875889.
Underwood, M. K. (2004). Glares of contempt, eye rolls of disgust and turning away to exclude: Non-verbal forms of social aggression among girls. Feminism Psychology, 14, 371–375. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959353504044637.
Wagner, H. L. (1993). On measuring performance in category judgment studies of nonverbal behavior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987006.
Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Children’s recognition of disgust in others. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 271–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031640.
Zuckerman, M., Lipets, M. S., Hall Koivumaki, J., & Rosenthal, R. (1975). Encoding and decoding nonverbal cues of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1068–1076. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.32.6.1068.
Zupan, B. (2015). Recognition of high and low intensity facial and vocal expressions of emotion by children and adults. Journal of Social Sciences and Humanities, 1, 332–344. Retrieved from: http://files.aiscience.org/journal/article/html/70320049.html.
Funding
This work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the McGill Research Ethics Board, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Appendix: Full Factorial ANOVA Model Results
Appendix: Full Factorial ANOVA Model Results
Vocal cues | df | F | η 2 | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Listener age | (1, 132) | 14.49 | .10 | < .001 |
Listener gender | (1, 132) | 10.42 | .07 | < .01 |
Speaker age | (1, 132) | 140.71 | .52 | < .001 |
Speaker gender | (1, 132) | 4.84 | .04 | < .05 |
Expression | (5.28, 696.76) | 130.89 | .50 | < .001 |
Speaker age × listener age | (1, 132) | 0.23 | < .01 | .64 |
Speaker gender × listener gender | (1, 132) | 0.01 | < .001 | .93 |
Listener age × listener gender | (1, 129) | 0.09 | .001 | .76 |
Listener age × speaker gender | (1, 132) | 4.19 | .03 | .04 |
Speaker age × listener gender | (1, 132) | 0.12 | .001 | .73 |
Speaker age × speaker gender | (1, 132) | 2.03 | .02 | .16 |
Expression × listener age | (5.28, 696.96) | 3.54 | .03 | < .01 |
Expression × listener gender | (5.28, 696.96) | 0.54 | < .01 | .76 |
Expression × speaker age | (5.25, 692.39) | 12.02 | .08 | < .001 |
Expression × speaker gender | (5.34, 705.42) | 27.49 | .17 | < .001 |
Expression × listener age × listener gender | (5.28, 681.12) | 1.93 | .01 | .08 |
Speaker age × listener age × listener gender | (1, 129) | 0.18 | .001 | .67 |
Speaker gender × listener age × listener gender | (1, 129) | 0.93 | .01 | .34 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × listener age | (1, 132) | 1.65 | .01 | .20 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × listener gender | (1, 132) | 0.80 | .01 | .37 |
Speaker age × expression × listener age | (5.25, 696.96) | 3.55 | .03 | < .01 |
Speaker age × expression × listener gender | (5.25, 696.96) | 1.68 | .01 | .13 |
Speaker gender × expression × listener age | (5.34, 696.96) | 1.53 | .01 | .17 |
Speaker gender × expression × listener gender | (5.34, 696.96) | 1.60 | .01 | .15 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × expression | (5.08, 670.26) | 11.04 | .08 | < .001 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × listener age × listener gender | (1, 129) | 3.16 | .02 | .08 |
Speaker age × expression × listener age × listener gender | (5.25, 681.12) | 0.25 | < .01 | .95 |
Speaker gender × expression × listener age × listener gender | (5.34, 681.12) | 0.95 | .01 | .45 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × expression × listener age | (5.08, 696.96) | 1.19 | .01 | .31 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × expression × listener gender | (5.08, 696.96) | 1.37 | .01 | .23 |
Speaker age × speaker gender × expression × listener age × listener gender | (5.08, 681.12) | 1.01 | .01 | .41 |
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Morningstar, M., Ly, V.Y., Feldman, L. et al. Mid-Adolescents’ and Adults’ Recognition of Vocal Cues of Emotion and Social Intent: Differences by Expression and Speaker Age. J Nonverbal Behav 42, 237–251 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-0274-7
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-018-0274-7